Anti-pointing is mediated by a perceptual bias of target location in left and right visual space

We sought to determine whether mirror-symmetrical limb movements (so-called anti-pointing) elicit a pattern of endpoint bias commensurate with perceptual judgments. In particular, we examined whether asymmetries related to the perceptual over- and under-estimation of target extent in respective left and right visual space impacts the trajectories of anti-pointing. In Experiment 1, participants completed direct (i.e. pro-pointing) and mirror-symmetrical (i.e. anti-pointing) responses to targets in left and right visual space with their right hand. In line with the anti-saccade literature, anti-pointing yielded longer reaction times than pro-pointing: a result suggesting increased top-down processing for the sensorimotor transformations underlying a mirror-symmetrical response. Most interestingly, pro-pointing yielded comparable endpoint accuracy in left and right visual space; however, anti-pointing produced an under- and overshooting bias in respective left and right visual space. In Experiment 2, we replicated the findings from Experiment 1 and further demonstrate that the endpoint bias of anti-pointing is independent of the reaching limb (i.e. left vs. right hand) and between-task differences in saccadic drive. We thus propose that the visual field-specific endpoint bias observed here is related to the cognitive (i.e. top-down) nature of anti-pointing and the corollary use of visuo-perceptual networks to support the sensorimotor transformations underlying such actions.

[1]  J. R. Simon,et al.  CHOICE REACTION TIME AS A FUNCTION OF ANGULAR STIMULUS-RESPONSE CORRESPONDENCE AND AGE , 1963 .

[2]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Can the motor system utilize a stored representation to control movement , 2004 .

[3]  James Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  Richard Held,et al.  Technique for Studying Adaptation to Disarranged Hand-Eye Coordination , 1958 .

[5]  M. Nicholls,et al.  Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgement of brightness, numerosity and size , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  M. Bryden Measuring handedness with questionnaires , 1977, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Antisaccade performance predicted by neuronal activity in the supplementary eye field , 1997, Nature.

[8]  James L. Lyons,et al.  Optimal Control Strategies Under Different Feedback Schedules: Kinematic Evidence , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  Matti S. Hämäläinen,et al.  Where left becomes right: A magnetoencephalographic study of sensorimotor transformation for antisaccades , 2007, NeuroImage.

[10]  M. Arbib,et al.  Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  Jun Zhang,et al.  Flexible filaments in a flowing soap film as a model for one-dimensional flags in a two-dimensional wind , 2000, Nature.

[12]  B. Day,et al.  Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[13]  J. D. Fisk,et al.  The organization of eye and limb movements during unrestricted reaching to targets in contralateral and ipsilateral visual space , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  E A Roy,et al.  The accuracy of reaching movements in brief delay conditions. , 2001, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[15]  Y. Joanette,et al.  Visual allesthesia in manual pointing: Some evidence for a sensorimotor cerebral organization , 1984, Brain and Cognition.

[16]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Grasping after a Delay Shifts Size-Scaling from Absolute to Relative Metrics , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  T. Vilis,et al.  Remapping the remembered target location for anti-saccades in human posterior parietal cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  K. Luh Line bisection and perceptual asymmetries in normal individuals: What you see is not what you get. , 1995 .

[19]  Philip N. Sabes,et al.  Flexible strategies for sensory integration during motor planning , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Utilization of Visual Feedback Information during Rapid Pointing Movements , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[22]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Proctor,et al.  Processing of an irrelevant location dimension as a function of the relevant stimulus dimension. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  A comparison of frontoparietal fMRI activation during anti-saccades and anti-pointing. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Dependence of saccadic eye-movements on stimulus luminance, and an effect of task , 1988, Vision Research.

[26]  Melvyn A. Goodale,et al.  Reaching to ipsilateral or contralateral targets: within-hemisphere visuomotor processing cannot explain hemispatial differences in motor control , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[28]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[29]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Goal-directed reaching: movement strategies influence the weighting of allocentric and egocentric visual cues , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Egocentric and Allocentric Visual Cues Influence the Specification of Movement Distance and Direction , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.

[31]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The control of memory-guided reaching movements in peripersonal space. , 2004, Motor control.

[32]  M. Heath,et al.  Visuomotor memory is independent of conscious awareness of target features , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  Nikolaos Smyrnis,et al.  The mirror antisaccade task: direction–amplitude interaction and spatial accuracy characteristics , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Memory-Guided Reaching: What the Visuomotor System Knows and How Long It Knows It , 2010 .

[35]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[36]  Peter McGeorge,et al.  Hemispatial asymmetries in judgment of stimulus size , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.

[37]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades , 2000, Nature.

[38]  D P Munoz,et al.  Role of Primate Superior Colliculus in Preparation and Execution of Anti-Saccades and Pro-Saccades , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Munoz,et al.  Reflex suppression in the anti-saccade task is dependent on prestimulus neural processes. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  D. Westwood,et al.  Action control: Independent effects of memory and monocular viewing on reaching accuracy , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[41]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al.  Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[43]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The proximity of visual landmarks impacts reaching performance. , 2007, Spatial vision.

[44]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[45]  S. Glover,et al.  Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[46]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  Persistent LIP activity in memory antisaccades: working memory for a sensorimotor transformation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Visuomotor system uses target features unavailable to conscious awareness , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[49]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Can a visual representation support the online control of memory-dependent reaching? Evident from a variable spatial mapping paradigm. , 2003, Motor control.

[50]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Background visual cues and memory-guided reaching. , 2004, Human movement science.

[51]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[52]  Romeo Chua,et al.  Asymmetries in the spatial localization of transformed targets , 1992, Brain and Cognition.

[53]  M. Nicholls,et al.  Are object- and space-based attentional biases both important to free-viewing perceptual asymmetries? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[54]  Erik J Schlicht,et al.  Impact of coordinate transformation uncertainty on human sensorimotor control. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  M. Goodale,et al.  Perceptual illusion and the real-time control of action. , 2003, Spatial vision.

[56]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  D. Westwood,et al.  No Evidence for Accurate Visuomotor Memory: Systematic and Variable Error in Memory-Guided Reaching , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[58]  D. Grossi,et al.  Left on the Right or Viceversa: A Case of “Alternating” Constructional Allochiria , 2004, Cortex.

[59]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Ocular perturbations and retinal/extraretinal information: the coordination of saccadic and manual movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[60]  Matthew Heath,et al.  Visuomotor Memory for Target Location in Near and Far Reaching Spaces , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.

[61]  J. Vercher,et al.  The role of ocular muscle proprioception in visual localization of targets. , 1990, Science.

[62]  B. Fischer,et al.  Characteristics of “anti” saccades in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  Digby Elliott,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Motor Performance , 1996 .

[64]  S. Barthelemy,et al.  Manual asymmetries in the directional coding of reaching: further evidence for hemispatial effects and right hemisphere dominance for movement planning , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[65]  J. Crawford,et al.  Gaze-Centered Remapping of Remembered Visual Space in an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  M. Heath Role of limb and target vision in the online control of memory-guided reaches. , 2005, Motor control.

[67]  Tony Ro,et al.  Maintenance of Visual Stability in the Human Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  M. Heath,et al.  Can the motor system resolve a premovement bias in grip aperture? Online analysis of grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[69]  A. Milner,et al.  To halve and to halve not: An analysis of line bisection judgements in normal subjects , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  Kristen A. Ford,et al.  Effect of stimulus probability on anti-saccade error rates , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[71]  P. Haggard,et al.  Action and awareness in pointing tasks , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[72]  J. Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[73]  A. Milner,et al.  Visually guided reaching: bilateral posterior parietal lesions cause a switch from fast visuomotor to slow cognitive control , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[74]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two distinct modes of control for object-directed action. , 2004, Progress in brain research.

[75]  D Elliott,et al.  Monocular and Binocular Vision in the Control of Goal-Directed Movement , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.

[76]  M. Heath,et al.  Visual feedback schedules influence visuomotor resistance to the Müller-Lyer figures , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[77]  J. Cullen,et al.  Manual Asymmetries in Goal-Directed Movement: Examination of the Motor Output Hypothesis , 1998, Brain and Cognition.