Dynamics of saccade target selection: Race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque

We investigated how saccade target selection by humans and macaque monkeys reacts to unexpected changes of the image. This was explored using double step and search step tasks in which a target, presented alone or as a singleton in a visual search array, steps to a different location on infrequent, random trials. We report that human and macaque monkey performance are qualitatively indistinguishable. Performance is stochastic with the probability of producing a compensated saccade to the final target location decreasing with the delay of the step. Compensated saccades to the final target location are produced with latencies relative to the step that are comparable to or less than the average latency of saccades on trials with no target step. Noncompensated errors to the initial target location are produced with latencies less than the average latency of saccades on trials with no target step. Noncompensated saccades to the initial target location are followed by corrective saccades to the final target location following an intersaccade interval that decreases with the interval between the target step and the initiation of the noncompensated saccade. We show that this pattern of results cannot be accounted for by a race between two stochastically independent processes producing the saccade to the initial target location and another process producing the saccade to the final target location. However, performance can be accounted for by a race between three stochastically independent processes--a GO process producing the saccade to the initial target location, a STOP process interrupting that GO process, and another GO process producing the saccade to the final target location. Furthermore, if the STOP process and second GO process start at the same time, then the model can account for the incidence and latency of mid-flight corrections and rapid corrective saccades. This model provides a computational account of saccade production when the image changes unexpectedly.

[1]  Kuniharu Arai,et al.  A model of the saccade-generating system that accounts for trajectory variations produced by competing visual stimuli , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[2]  M. Paré,et al.  Guidance of eye movements during visual conjunction search: local and global contextual effects on target discriminability. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  A. J. Van Opstal,et al.  Three-dimensional analysis of strongly curved saccades elicited by double-step stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  R. Aslin,et al.  The amplitude and angle of saccades to double-step target displacements , 1987, Vision Research.

[5]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Countermanding saccades: Evidence against independent processing of go and stop signals , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Iain D Gilchrist,et al.  A population coding account for systematic variation in saccadic dead time. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Effect of target-distractor similarity on FEF visual selection in the absence of the target , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  J. Theeuwes Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  R. Andersen,et al.  Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex. , 2002, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[12]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The effect of frontal eye field and superior colliculus lesions on saccadic latencies in the rhesus monkey. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[14]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sequential activity of simultaneously recorded neurons in the superior colliculus during curved saccades. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  J M Findlay,et al.  Visual computation and saccadic eye movements: a theoretical perspective. , 1987, Spatial vision.

[16]  F. Ottes,et al.  Metrics of saccade responses to visual double stimuli: Two different modes , 1984, Vision Research.

[17]  Neeraj J Gandhi,et al.  Simulations of saccade curvature by models that place superior colliculus upstream from the local feedback loop. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  L. Festinger,et al.  Some observations concerning saccadic eye movements. , 1973, Vision research.

[19]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[20]  R. Andersen,et al.  Inactivation of macaque lateral intraparietal area delays initiation of the second saccade predominantly from contralesional eye positions in a double-saccade task , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  L. Optican,et al.  Model of the control of saccades by superior colliculus and cerebellum. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  J. Wolfe,et al.  What Can 1 Million Trials Tell Us About Visual Search? , 1998 .

[23]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[24]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[25]  A. V. van Opstal,et al.  Dynamic ensemble coding of saccades in the monkey superior colliculus. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Rafal Bogacz,et al.  Parameterization of connectionist models , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[27]  D. Meyer,et al.  The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stages of response preparation. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Saccade target selection in macaque during feature and conjunction visual search , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[29]  G. Logan,et al.  Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.

[30]  T. Zandt,et al.  How to fit a response time distribution , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[31]  Aditya Murthy,et al.  Frontal eye field contributions to rapid corrective saccades. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Effect of mean reaction time on saccadic responses to two-step stimuli with horizontal and vertical components , 1975, Vision Research.

[33]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Voluntary saccadic eye movements in humans studied with a double-cue paradigm , 2002, Vision Research.

[34]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[35]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  Hans Colonius,et al.  Countermanding saccades with auditory stop signals: testing the race model , 2001, Vision Research.

[37]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[38]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  T. Rowan Functional stability analysis of numerical algorithms , 1990 .

[40]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[41]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[42]  A. Murthy,et al.  Programming of double-step saccade sequences: Modulation by cognitive control , 2004, Vision Research.

[43]  A. Opstal,et al.  STIMULUS-INDUCED MIDFLIGHT MODIFICATION OF SACCADE TRAJECTORIES , 1987 .

[44]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Dependence and Independence in Responding to Double Stimulation : A Comparison of Stop , Change , and Dual-Task Paradigms , 1986 .

[45]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  J D Schall,et al.  Dynamic dissociation of visual selection from saccade programming in frontal eye field. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  Dependence and independence in responding to double stimulation: A comparison of stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. , 1986 .

[49]  H. Akaike,et al.  Information Theory and an Extension of the Maximum Likelihood Principle , 1973 .

[50]  F. Tuerlinckx The efficient computation of the cumulative distribution and probability density functions in the diffusion model , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[51]  Robert M McPeek,et al.  Competition between saccade goals in the superior colliculus produces saccade curvature. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Comparison of saccadic eye movements in humans and macaques to single-step and double-step target movements , 1989, Vision Research.

[53]  G D Logan,et al.  Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[55]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Pre-excitatory pause in frontal eye field responses , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[56]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Countermanding saccades in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[57]  John A. Nelder,et al.  A Simplex Method for Function Minimization , 1965, Comput. J..

[58]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The role of stimulus-driven and goal-driven control in saccadic visual selection. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[59]  James T. Townsend,et al.  The Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes , 1983 .