Beyond single syllables: Large-scale modeling of reading aloud with the Connectionist Dual Process (CDP++) model
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Michael Hammond,et al. The phonology of English : a prosodic optimality-theoretic approach , 1999 .
[2] Rebecca Treiman,et al. The English Lexicon Project , 2007, Behavior research methods.
[3] Arthur M. Jacobs,et al. Item performance in visual word recognition , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[4] Derek Besner,et al. When parallel processing in visual word recognition is not enough: New evidence from naming , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] A. Jacobs,et al. Models of visual word recognition: Sampling the state of the art. , 1994 .
[6] L. Colombo. Lexical stress effect and its interaction with frequency in word pronunciation , 1992 .
[7] Padraic Monaghan,et al. Stressing what is important: Orthographic cues and lexical stress assignment , 2009, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[8] P. Ladefoged. A course in phonetics , 1975 .
[9] David C. Plaut,et al. A connectionist approach to word reading and acquired dyslexia: extension to sequential processing , 1999, Cogn. Sci..
[10] A. Roelofs,et al. Metrical structure in planning the production of spoken words , 1998 .
[11] K. Rayner. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. , 1998, Psychological bulletin.
[12] M. H. Kelly,et al. Orthographic cues to lexical stress: Effects on naming and lexical decision , 1998, Memory & cognition.
[13] Marco Zorzi,et al. NORMAL AND IMPAIRED SPELLING IN A CONNECTIONIST DUAL-ROUTE ARCHITECTURE , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[14] Debra Jared,et al. Spelling-Sound Consistency Affects the Naming of High-Frequency Words , 1997 .
[15] Michael Hammond,et al. Vowel quantity and syllabification in English , 1997 .
[16] R. Ratcliff,et al. A Diffusion Model Account of Criterion Shifts in the Lexical Decision Task. , 2008, Journal of memory and language.
[17] R. Glushko. The Organization and Activation of Orthographic Knowledge in Reading Aloud. , 1979 .
[18] David S. Touretzky,et al. Connectionist Models and Linguistic Theory: Investigations of Stress Systems in Language , 1993, Cogn. Sci..
[19] S. Lupker,et al. Can CANISO activate CASINO? Transposed-letter similarity effects with nonadjacent letter positions ☆ , 2004 .
[20] References , 1971 .
[21] D. Share. Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition , 1995, Cognition.
[22] M Taft,et al. Processing of Orthographic Structure by Adults of Different Reading Ability , 2001, Language and speech.
[23] J. Ziegler,et al. Rules versus statistics in reading aloud: New evidence on an old debate , 2010 .
[24] Jyotsna Vaid,et al. Word frequency modulates the Basic Orthographic Syllabic Structure (BOSS) effect in English polysyllable word recognition , 2007 .
[25] A G Barto,et al. Toward a modern theory of adaptive networks: expectation and prediction. , 1981, Psychological review.
[26] M. Coltheart,et al. Lexical and Nonlexical Print-to-Sound Translation of Disyllabic Words and Nonwords , 2000 .
[27] M. Zorzi,et al. Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .
[28] Max Coltheart,et al. Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .
[29] Manuel Perea,et al. Transposed-letter effects: Consonants, vowels and letter frequency , 2008 .
[30] David A. Balota,et al. Bringing Computational Models of Word Naming Down to the Item Level , 1997 .
[31] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and Performance VI , 1978 .
[32] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .
[33] M. Zorzi. Serial processing in reading aloud: no challenge for a parallel model. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] Michael J Cortese,et al. Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[35] Boris New,et al. Syllabic length effects in visual word recognition and naming. , 2003, Acta psychologica.
[36] Daragh E. Sibley,et al. Learning orthographic and phonological representations in models of monosyllabic and bisyllabic naming , 2010 .
[37] M. van Oostendorp,et al. Schwa in phonological theory , 1998 .
[38] Lucia Colombo,et al. Interacting sources of information in word naming: a study of individual differences , 1994 .
[39] J Grainger,et al. Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model. , 1996, Psychological review.
[40] B. Ans,et al. A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. , 1998, Psychological review.
[41] Marcus Taft,et al. Lexical access-via an orthographic code: The basic orthographic syllabic structure (BOSS) , 1979 .
[42] Marco Zorzi,et al. Additive and interactive effects of stimulus degradation: no challenge for CDP+. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[43] F. L. Sack. English Word-Stress , 1969 .
[44] K. Paap,et al. Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race , 1991 .
[45] M. Behrmann,et al. Surface dyslexia and dysgraphia: dual routes, single lexicon , 1992 .
[46] James L. McClelland,et al. Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.
[47] J. Ziegler,et al. The Feedback Consistency Effect in Lexical Decision and Naming , 1997 .
[48] J. Mehler,et al. A destressing deafness in French , 1997 .
[49] Dan Chateau,et al. Spelling–sound consistency effects in disyllabic word naming , 2003 .
[50] Mark Yates,et al. Phonological neighbors speed visual word processing: evidence from multiple tasks. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[51] S. Rixon. English Phonetics and Phonology , 2003 .
[52] Curt Burgess,et al. Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence , 1996 .
[53] B. Weekes. Differential Effects of Number of Letters on Word and Nonword Naming Latency , 1997 .
[54] D. Besner,et al. Reading pseudohomophones: Implications for models of pronunciation assembly and the locus of word-frequency effects in naming. , 1987 .
[55] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Simulating consistency effects and individual differences in nonword naming: A comparison of current models , 2006 .
[56] Derek Besner,et al. Neighborhood effects in reading aloud: new findings and new challenges for computational models. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] J. Ziegler,et al. Identical Words are Read Differently in Different Languages , 2001, Psychological science.
[58] Sally Andrews,et al. Rule and analogy mechanisms in reading nonwords: Hough dou peapel rede gnew wirds? , 1998 .
[59] Sally Andrews,et al. From inkmarks to ideas : current issues in lexical processing , 2006 .
[60] M Behrmann,et al. Frequency and consistency effects in a pure surface dyslexic patient. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[61] Linda Cupples,et al. Effects of Stress Typicality During Speeded Grammatical Classification , 2003, Language and speech.
[62] Johannes C. Ziegler,et al. A developmental perspective on the neural code for written words , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[63] M. Zorzi. The connectionist dual process (CDP) approach to modelling reading aloud , 2010 .
[64] R. H. Baayen,et al. The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .
[65] P. Carr. English phonetics and phonology , 1999 .
[66] C. J. Álvarez,et al. Syllables and morphemes: contrasting frequency effects in Spanish. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[67] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Computing the meanings of words in reading: cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. , 2004, Psychological review.
[68] M. H. Kelly,et al. Word Onset Patterns and Lexical Stress in English. , 2004 .
[69] Ludovic Ferrand,et al. Reading aloud polysyllabic words and nonwords: The syllabic length effect reexamined , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[70] Iggy Roca,et al. Derivations and constraints in phonology , 1999 .
[71] Arthur M. Jacobs,et al. Word, Pseudoword, and Nonword Processing: A Multitask Comparison Using Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[72] Marco Zorzi,et al. Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. , 2007, Psychological review.
[73] Bernard Widrow,et al. Adaptive switching circuits , 1988 .
[74] Barry Heselwood,et al. Schwa and the phonotactics of RP English , 2007 .
[75] T. A. Hall,et al. English syllabification as the interaction of markedness constraints , 2006 .
[76] D. Feldman-Stewart,et al. Learning in small connectionist networks does not generalize to large networks , 1994 .
[77] Jeffrey S Bowers,et al. Contrasting five different theories of letter position coding: evidence from orthographic similarity effects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[78] Edward Flemming,et al. Rosa's roses: reduced vowels in American English , 2004, Journal of the International Phonetic Association.
[79] Larry E. Toothaker,et al. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions , 1991 .
[80] M. Gordon. Syllable Weight: Phonetics, Phonology, Typology , 2006 .
[81] Michael C. Doyle,et al. Effects of frequency on visual word recognition tasks: where are they? , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[82] B. Hayes. Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies , 1995 .
[83] J. Ziegler,et al. Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.
[84] T. A. Hall. Against extrasyllabic consonants in German and English , 2002, Phonology.
[85] Derek Besner,et al. The cross-script length effect: further evidence challenging PDP models of reading aloud. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[86] D. Balota,et al. Visual word recognition of multisyllabic words , 2009 .
[87] D. Jared. Spelling-Sound Consistency and Regularity Effects in Word Naming , 2002 .
[88] C. Clifton,et al. The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements during silent reading , 2005, Cognition.
[89] Usha Goswami,et al. Phonology, reading development, and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective , 2002 .
[90] K. Rayner. Eye movements in reading and information processing. , 1978, Psychological bulletin.
[91] R Treiman,et al. Children's sensitivity to syllables, onsets, rimes, and phonemes. , 1996, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[92] James L. McClelland,et al. Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. , 1987 .
[93] M. Coltheart,et al. Serial and strategic effects in reading aloud , 1999 .
[94] Conrad Perry,et al. Speed of lexical and nonlexical processing in French: The case of the regularity effect , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[95] Michael Hammond,et al. The phonology of English , 1999 .
[96] Marco Zorzi,et al. The Development of Spelling-Sound Relationships in a Model of Phonological Reading , 1998 .
[97] James L. McClelland,et al. Nonword pronunciation and models of word recognition. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[98] J. Bullinaria. Modeling Reading, Spelling, and Past Tense Learning with Artificial Neural Networks , 1997, Brain and Language.
[99] Linda Cupples,et al. The processing of lexical stress during visual word recognition: Typicality effects and orthographic correlates , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[100] Arthur M. Jacobs,et al. What is the pronunciation for -ough and the spelling for /u/? A database for computing feedforward and feedback consistency in English , 1997 .
[101] Rebecca Treiman,et al. Phonetic Biases in Voice Key Response Time Measurements , 2002 .
[102] L. Allan,et al. The widespread influence of the Rescorla-Wagner model , 1996, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[103] J. Ziegler,et al. Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating individual differences and subtypes , 2008, Cognition.
[104] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Naming multisyllabic words. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[105] Chris Davis,et al. The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: interference effects from a masked prime , 1991 .
[106] S. Hains,et al. Individual differences in word recognition latency , 1979 .
[107] Marco Zorzi,et al. Do current connectionist learning models account for reading development in different languages? , 2004, Cognition.
[108] Ken N. Seergobin,et al. On the association between connectionism and data: Are a few words necessary? , 1990 .
[109] Alan H. Kawamoto,et al. Pronunciation of homographs , 1992 .
[110] Barry Heselwood,et al. R vocalisation, linking R and intrusive R: accounting for final schwa in RP English , 2009 .
[111] S. Lupker,et al. Strategic Control in a Naming Task: Changing Routes or Changing Deadlines? , 1997 .
[112] S. Andrews. Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Activation or search? , 1989 .
[113] Conrad Perry,et al. Testing a Computational Account of Category-Specific Deficits , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[114] D. Balota,et al. Moving beyond Coltheart’s N: A new measure of orthographic similarity , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[115] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[116] James L. McClelland,et al. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.
[117] Alain Content,et al. The effect of spelling-to-sound regularity on naming in French , 1991 .
[118] A. Caramazza,et al. The assignment of word stress in oral reading: Evidence from a case of acquired dyslexia. , 1993 .
[119] David A. Balota,et al. The Utility of Item-Level Analyses in Model Evaluation: A Reply to Seidenberg and Plaut , 1998 .
[120] S. Becker,et al. Long-term semantic priming: a computational account and empirical evidence. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[121] M Coltheart,et al. DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.