Sequential egocentric strategy is acquired as early as allocentric strategy: Parallel acquisition of these two navigation strategies

At least two main cognitive strategies can be used to solve a complex navigation task: the allocentric or map‐based strategy and the sequential egocentric or route‐based strategy. The sequential egocentric strategy differs from a succession of independent simple egocentric responses as it requires a sequential ordering of events, possibly sharing functional similarity with episodic memory in this regard. To question the possible simultaneous encoding of sequential egocentric and allocentric strategies, we developed a paradigm in which these two strategies are spontaneously used or imposed. Our results evidenced that sequential egocentric strategy can be spontaneously acquired at the onset of the training as well as allocentric strategy. Allocentric and sequential egocentric strategies could be used together within a trial, and bidirectional shifts (between trials) were spontaneously performed during the training period by 30% of the participants. Regardless of the strategy used spontaneously during the training, all participants could execute immediate shifts to the opposite non previously used strategy when this strategy was imposed. Altogether, our findings suggest that subjects acquire different types of spatial knowledge in parallel, namely knowledge permitting allocentric navigation as well as knowledge permitting sequential egocentric navigation. © 2009 Wiley‐Liss, Inc.

[1]  M. Petrides,et al.  Cognitive Strategies Dependent on the Hippocampus and Caudate Nucleus in Human Navigation: Variability and Change with Practice , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  A Berthoz,et al.  [Neural basis of spatial orientation and memory of routes: topokinetic memory or topokinesthesic memory]. , 2001, Revue neurologique.

[3]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Complementary Roles of Hippocampus and Medial Entorhinal Cortex in Episodic Memory , 2008, Neural plasticity.

[4]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Mental navigation along memorized routes activates the hippocampus, precuneus, and insula , 1997, Neuroreport.

[5]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection , 2001 .

[6]  Emilio Kropff,et al.  Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  A. Etienne,et al.  Navigation through vector addition , 1998, Nature.

[8]  M. Potegal The caudate nucleus egocentric localization system. , 1972, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[9]  R. Morris,et al.  Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions , 1982, Nature.

[10]  A Berthoz,et al.  Parietal and hippocampal contribution to topokinetic and topographic memory. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by Path Integration , 1982 .

[12]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled Distinct Neural Bases of Route Following and Wayfinding in Humans , 2003, Neuron.

[13]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Inactivation of Hippocampus or Caudate Nucleus with Lidocaine Differentially Affects Expression of Place and Response Learning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[14]  E. Tolman Cognitive maps in rats and men. , 1948, Psychological review.

[15]  Giuseppe Iaria,et al.  Hippocampal function and spatial memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging in healthy participants and performance of patients with medial temporal lobe resections. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[16]  Guillén Fernández,et al.  Interaction between the Human Hippocampus and the Caudate Nucleus during Route Recognition , 2004, Neuron.

[17]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. , 1998, Science.

[18]  E. Rolls,et al.  A computational theory of hippocampal function, and empirical tests of the theory , 2006, Progress in Neurobiology.

[19]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  Impaired Sequential Egocentric and Allocentric Memories in Forebrain-Specific–NMDA Receptor Knock-Out Mice during a New Task Dissociating Strategies of Navigation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Angelo Arleo,et al.  Multimodal sensory integration and concurrent navigation strategies for spatial cognition in real and artificial organisms. , 2007, Journal of integrative neuroscience.

[21]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[22]  H. Eichenbaum The hippocampus and declarative memory: cognitive mechanisms and neural codes , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.

[23]  R. Astur,et al.  Sex differences and correlations in a virtual Morris water task, a virtual radial arm maze, and mental rotation , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  Multiple Parallel Memory Systems in the Brain of the Rat , 2002, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[25]  Nicole Etchamendy,et al.  Spontaneous navigational strategies and performance in the virtual town , 2007, Hippocampus.

[26]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Combination of conflicting visual and non-visual information for estimating actively performed body turns in virtual reality. , 2003, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[27]  E. Maguire,et al.  The Human Hippocampus and Spatial and Episodic Memory , 2002, Neuron.

[28]  S. Bricogne,et al.  Neural Correlates of Topographic Mental Exploration: The Impact of Route versus Survey Perspective Learning , 2000, NeuroImage.

[29]  D. Hassabis,et al.  Using Imagination to Understand the Neural Basis of Episodic Memory , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  S J Mizumori,et al.  Hippocampal Representational Organization and Spatial Context , 1999, Hippocampus.

[31]  N. Burgess,et al.  Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  H. Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by path integration in a mammal , 1980, Naturwissenschaften.