The Biological Bases of Conformity

Humans are characterized by an extreme dependence on culturally transmitted information and recent formal theory predicts that natural selection should favor adaptive learning strategies that facilitate effective copying and decision making. One strategy that has attracted particular attention is conformist transmission, defined as the disproportionately likely adoption of the most common variant. Conformity has historically been emphasized as significant in the social psychology literature, and recently there have also been reports of conformist behavior in non-human animals. However, mathematical analyses differ in how important and widespread they expect conformity to be, and relevant experimental work is scarce, and generates findings that are both mutually contradictory and inconsistent with the predictions of the models. We review the relevant literature considering the causation, function, history, and ontogeny of conformity, and describe a computer-based experiment on human subjects that we carried out in order to resolve ambiguities. We found that only when many demonstrators were available and subjects were uncertain was subject behavior conformist. A further analysis found that the underlying response to social information alone was generally conformist. Thus, our data are consistent with a conformist use of social information, but as subjects’ behavior is the result of both social and asocial influences, the resultant behavior may not be conformist. We end by relating these findings to an embryonic cognitive neuroscience literature that has recently begun to explore the neural bases of social learning. Here conformist transmission may be a particularly useful case study, not only because there are well-defined and tractable opportunities to characterize the biological underpinnings of this form of social learning, but also because early findings imply that humans may possess specific cognitive adaptations for effective social learning.

[1]  J. Campbell,et al.  Informational and normative routes to conformity: The effect of faction size as a function of norm extremity and attention to the stimulus. , 1989 .

[2]  Kevin N. Laland,et al.  Conformist learning in nine-spined sticklebacks' foraging decisions , 2010, Biology Letters.

[3]  M. Deutsch,et al.  A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgement. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[4]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.

[5]  K. Laland,et al.  From fish to fashion: experimental and theoretical insights into the evolution of culture , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  Effects of strength and immediacy in group contexts: Reply to Jackson. , 1986 .

[7]  Kathleen H Corriveau,et al.  Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[8]  Mark Lubell,et al.  Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission , 2008 .

[9]  Martin H. Levinson Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution , 2006 .

[10]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of cultural evolution , 2003 .

[11]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Mark W Woolrich,et al.  Associative learning of social value , 2008, Nature.

[13]  L. Toledo-Pereyra Trust , 2006, Mediation Behaviour.

[14]  B. Galef,et al.  ‘Conformity’ in Norway rats? , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  Paul L. Harris,et al.  The Basis of Epistemic Trust: Reliable Testimony or Reliable Sources? , 2007, Episteme.

[16]  E. M. Tong,et al.  Conformity: moods matter , 2008 .

[17]  M. Feldman,et al.  Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. , 1981, Monographs in population biology.

[18]  A. Whiten,et al.  Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees , 2005, Nature.

[19]  K. Laland Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[20]  P. Harris,et al.  Preschoolers continue to trust a more accurate informant 1 week after exposure to accuracy information. , 2009, Developmental science.

[21]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[22]  R. Boyd,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. , 1998 .

[23]  Magnus Enquist,et al.  Social Learning : A Solution to Rogers ’ s Paradox of Nonadaptive Culture , 2007 .

[24]  P. Harris,et al.  Going With the Flow , 2009, Psychological science.

[25]  B. Latané,et al.  The Social Impact of Majorities and Minorities. , 1981 .

[26]  K. Laland,et al.  The evolutionary basis of human social learning , 2012, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[27]  K. Laland,et al.  Cause and Effect in Biology Revisited: Is Mayr's Proximate-ultimate , 2011 .

[28]  Malia Mason,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Social Influence , 2009 .

[29]  Magnus Enquist,et al.  CRITICAL POINTS IN CURRENT THEORY OF CONFORMIST SOCIAL LEARNING , 2007 .

[30]  R. Bond,et al.  Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies using Asch's (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. , 1996 .

[31]  P. Harris,et al.  Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. , 2005, Child development.

[32]  Yadin Dudai,et al.  Following the Crowd: Brain Substrates of Long-Term Memory Conformity , 2011, Science.

[33]  Luc-Alain Giraldeau,et al.  The evolution of social learning rules: payoff-biased and frequency-dependent biased transmission. , 2009, Journal of theoretical biology.

[34]  B. Mullen Strength and immediacy of sources: A meta-analytic evaluation of the forgotten elements of social impact theory. , 1985 .

[35]  Timothy M. Waring,et al.  Article in Press Evolution and Human Behavior Xxx (2005) Xxx – Xxx , 2022 .

[36]  M. Feldman,et al.  Individual Versus Social Learning: Evolutionary Analysis in a Fluctuating Environment , 1996 .

[37]  A. Jenness,et al.  The role of discussion in changing opinion regarding a matter of fact. , 1932 .

[38]  Caroline F. Zink,et al.  Neurobiological Correlates of Social Conformity and Independence During Mental Rotation , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[39]  S. Asch Opinions and Social Pressure , 1955, Nature.

[40]  In defense of Social Impact Theory: Comment on Mullen. , 1986 .

[41]  Robert S. Baron,et al.  The forgotten variable in conformity research: Impact of task importance on social influence. , 1996 .

[42]  R. Bond,et al.  Group Size and Conformity , 2005 .

[43]  Gregory S. Berns,et al.  Neural mechanisms of the influence of popularity on adolescent ratings of music , 2010, NeuroImage.

[44]  Muzafer Sherif,et al.  A study of some social factors in perception. , 1935 .

[45]  Karl H. Schlag,et al.  Which One Should I Imitate , 1999 .

[46]  Ulf Toelch,et al.  Humans copy rapidly increasing choices in a multiarmed bandit problem , 2010 .

[47]  E. Wilson,et al.  Genes, mind, and culture : the coevolutionary process , 1982 .

[48]  Kathleen H Corriveau,et al.  Young children's selective trust in informants , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[49]  Joe Yuichiro Wakano,et al.  Do social learning and conformist bias coevolve? Henrich and Boyd revisited. , 2007, Theoretical population biology.

[50]  G. Fernández,et al.  Reinforcement Learning Signal Predicts Social Conformity , 2009, Neuron.

[51]  Steven D. Penrod,et al.  Social Influence Model: A formal integration of research on majority and minority influence processes. , 1984 .

[52]  B. Latané,et al.  From private attitude to public opinion: A dynamic theory of social impact. , 1990 .

[53]  K. Schlag Why Imitate, and If So, How?, : A Boundedly Rational Approach to Multi-armed Bandits , 1998 .

[54]  P. Harris,et al.  Children assess informant reliability using bystanders' non-verbal cues. , 2008, Developmental science.

[55]  A. Whiten,et al.  In-Group Conformity Sustains Different Foraging Traditions in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella) , 2009, PloS one.

[56]  R. Dolan,et al.  How the Opinion of Others Affects Our Valuation of Objects , 2010, Current Biology.

[57]  Chad R. Mortensen,et al.  Going along versus going alone: when fundamental motives facilitate strategic (non)conformity. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[58]  G. Telegdy,et al.  Conformity in the Rat: A Leader's Selection of Door Color versus a Learned Door-Color Discrimination , 1977 .

[59]  Paul L. Harris,et al.  The Ontogenesis of Trust , 2004 .

[60]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  The effect of task structure on diffusion dynamics: Implications for diffusion curve and network-based analyses , 2010, Learning & behavior.

[61]  B. Latané The psychology of social impact. , 1981 .

[62]  R. L. Day,et al.  Interactions between shoal size and conformity in guppy social foraging , 2001, Animal Behaviour.