Attention capture without awareness in a non-spatial selection task

Distractors presented prior to a critical target in a rapid sequence of visually-presented items induce a lag-dependent deficit in target identification, particularly when the distractor shares a task-relevant feature of the target. Presumably, such capture of central attention is important for bringing a target into awareness. The results of the present investigation suggest that greater capture of attention by a distractor is not accompanied by greater awareness of it. Moreover, awareness tends to be limited to superficial characteristics of the target such as colour. The findings are interpreted within the context of a model that assumes sudden increases in arousal trigger selection of information for consolidation in working memory. In this conceptualization, prolonged analysis of distractor items sharing task-relevant features leads to larger target identification deficits (i.e., greater capture) but no increase in awareness.

[1]  N. Squires,et al.  The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.

[2]  Swann Pichon,et al.  Threat prompts defensive brain responses independently of attentional control. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Jane E. Raymond,et al.  Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation , 1994 .

[4]  S. Berti,et al.  Encoding of faces and objects into visual working memory: an event-related brain potential study , 2013, Neuroreport.

[5]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[6]  Dominique Lamy,et al.  The Role of Conscious Perception in Attentional Capture and Object-File Updating , 2015, Psychological science.

[7]  C. Chabris,et al.  Gorillas in Our Midst: Sustained Inattentional Blindness for Dynamic Events , 1999, Perception.

[8]  Neelam Naikar,et al.  A new component of visual orienting: Implicit effects of peripheral information and subthreshold cues on covert attention , 1999 .

[9]  E. Vogel,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for a Postperceptual Locus of Suppression during the Attentional Blink Time-based Attention and the Attentional Blink , 1998 .

[10]  R. Dolan,et al.  Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.

[11]  Marvin M. Chun,et al.  Attentional rubbernecking: Cognitive control and personality in emotion-induced blindness , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  A. Engel,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of the attentional blink phenomenon. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  E. Hess,et al.  Pupil Size in Relation to Mental Activity during Simple Problem-Solving , 1964, Science.

[14]  Karen M. Arnell,et al.  Blinded by Emotion: Target Misses Follow Attention Capture by Arousing Distractors in RSVP , 2007 .

[15]  Lars Schwabe,et al.  Emotional modulation of the attentional blink: The neural structures involved in capturing and holding attention , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  N. Lavie Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  M. Potter,et al.  A two-stage model for multiple target detection in rapid serial visual presentation. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Stephen D. Smith,et al.  The naked truth: Positive, arousing distractors impair rapid target perception , 2007 .

[19]  David Alais,et al.  No attentional capture from invisible flicker , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Top-down control settings and the attentional blink: Evidence for nonspatial contingent capture , 2008 .

[22]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  Made you blink! Contingent attentional capture produces a spatial blink , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[23]  R. Dolan,et al.  Effects of Attention and Emotion on Face Processing in the Human Brain An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2001, Neuron.

[24]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  R. Dolan,et al.  Amygdala Automaticity in Emotional Processing , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[26]  C. Olivers,et al.  A boost and bounce theory of temporal attention. , 2008, Psychological review.

[27]  Hermann J Müller,et al.  Gamma flicker triggers attentional selection without awareness , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[28]  K L Shapiro,et al.  Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? . , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  Shihui Han,et al.  Alerting and orienting of attention without visual awareness , 2012, Consciousness and Cognition.

[30]  Christopher L Asplund,et al.  Surprise-induced blindness: a stimulus-driven attentional limit to conscious perception. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  P. McCormick Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  Evian Gordon,et al.  A Direct Brainstem–amygdala–cortical Dalarmt System for Subliminal Signals of Fear , 2004 .

[33]  S. Murray,et al.  More Power to the Unconscious , 2015, Psychological science.

[34]  Helgi B. Schiöth,et al.  Exposure to subliminal arousing stimuli induces robust activation in the amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate, insular cortex and primary visual cortex: A systematic meta-analysis of fMRI studies , 2012, NeuroImage.

[35]  J. Enns,et al.  The attentional blink: Resource depletion or temporary loss of control? , 2005, Psychological research.

[36]  Walter Ritter,et al.  The N2 component elicited by stimulus matches and multiple targets , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[37]  Briana L. Kennedy,et al.  Emotion-induced blindness reflects competition at early and late processing stages: An ERP study , 2014, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[38]  J. Bradshaw,et al.  Load and pupillary changes in continuous processing tasks. , 1968, British journal of psychology.

[39]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  J. Hyönä,et al.  Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. , 2006, Emotion.

[41]  W. Singer,et al.  Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  M C Potter,et al.  Two attentional deficits in serial target search: the visual attentional blink and an amodal task-switch deficit. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[43]  Stephen D. Smith,et al.  An emotion-induced attentional blink elicited by aversively conditioned stimuli. , 2006, Emotion.

[44]  Arne Öhman,et al.  On the automatic nature of phobic fear: conditioned electrodermal responses to masked fear-relevant stimuli. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[45]  Ralph Adolphs,et al.  Processing of the Arousal of Subliminal and Supraliminal Emotional Stimuli by the Human Amygdala , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Pop-Out Without Awareness , 2011, Psychological science.

[47]  T. Lachmann,et al.  Event-related brain potentials dissociate visual working memory processes under categorial and identical comparison conditions. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[48]  E. Vogel,et al.  Delayed working memory consolidation during the attentional blink , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[49]  K. McDermott,et al.  Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. , 1995 .