Flexible foundations of abstract thought: A review and a theory

Since the proposal of conceptual metaphors as the representational means for grounding abstract concepts in concrete sensorio-motor experiences, experimental research about this issue is on the rise. The present paper identifies the problem of flexibility as one of the key questions that remains to receive a satisfactory answer, and proposes a psycho logically plausible model that offers such an answer. The model is grounded on basic spatial cognition principles, working memory representations and attentional processes. This framework integrates prior results and licenses several new predictions. Direct test of some of these predictions is provided by two recent studies from our lab. Finally, we discuss the implications of this framework for the issues of the manifestation of conceptual metaphors in behaviour, the acquisition of conceptual metaphors, their cross-cultural variation, and the Symbol Grounding Problem.

[1]  Edward Kako,et al.  Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001) , 2007, Cognition.

[2]  Jean Piaget,et al.  The child's conception of time; , 1969 .

[3]  Wim Fias,et al.  The Mental Representation of Ordinal Sequences is Spatially Organised: Evidence from Days of the Week , 2004, Cortex.

[4]  T. Boult,et al.  The eyes have it , 2003, WBMA '03.

[5]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  When "light" and "dark" thoughts become light and dark responses: affect biases brightness judgments. , 2007, Emotion.

[6]  P. Wilson Perceptual distortion of height as a function of ascribed academic status. , 1968, The Journal of social psychology.

[7]  J. Lammertyn,et al.  The hunt for SNARC , 2005 .

[8]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[9]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Thomas W. Schubert Your highness: vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  L. Elizabeth Crawford,et al.  Affect biases memory of location: Evidence for the spatial representation of affect , 2006 .

[12]  A. Markman,et al.  Constraining Theories of Embodied Cognition , 2005, Psychological science.

[13]  Marina Rakova,et al.  The philosophy of embodied realism: A high price to pay? , 2002 .

[14]  D. Slobin Thinking for Speaking , 1987 .

[15]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  S. Glucksberg,et al.  Conventional Language: How Metaphorical Is It? , 2000 .

[17]  D. Gentner,et al.  As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space → time metaphors , 2002 .

[18]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Frames of reference in vision and language: Where is above? , 1993, Cognition.

[19]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  John J. McCarthy,et al.  Optimality Theory: An overview , 2003 .

[21]  Brian F. Bowdle,et al.  The career of metaphor. , 2005, Psychological review.

[22]  P. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Preferred mental models in reasoning about spatial relations , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[23]  Rolf A. Zwaan The Immersed Experiencer: Toward An Embodied Theory Of Language Comprehension , 2003 .

[24]  Sarah F. Taub Iconicity in American sign language: concrete and metaphorical applications , 2000, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[25]  Irvin Rock,et al.  Orientation and form , 1974 .

[26]  B. Bergum,et al.  Attention and performance IX , 1982 .

[27]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  An effect of spatial–temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time , 2008, Cognition.

[28]  David H. Silvera,et al.  Judgment by Quantity , 1994 .

[29]  W. Bechtel,et al.  A companion to cognitive science , 1999 .

[30]  Alan Garnham A unified theory of the meaning of some spatial relational terms , 1989, Cognition.

[31]  S. Levinson,et al.  Returning the tables: language affects spatial reasoning , 2002, Cognition.

[32]  N. Sagiv,et al.  What is the relationship between synaesthesia and visuo-spatial number forms? , 2006, Cognition.

[33]  Effects of stimulus–stimulus short-term memory associations in a Simon-like task , 2008 .

[34]  R. Levine,et al.  A Geography Of Time , 1997 .

[35]  A. Henik,et al.  Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[36]  B. Landau,et al.  “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition , 1993 .

[37]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  A computational analysis of the apprehension of spatial relations , 1996 .

[38]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Reference Frame Activation during Spatial Term Assignment , 1994 .

[39]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Philosophy in the flesh : the embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought , 1999 .

[40]  N. Humphrey,et al.  Turning the Left Cheek , 1973, Nature.

[41]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .

[42]  A. Chatterjee,et al.  Verbs, events and spatial representations , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  L. Boroditsky,et al.  Time in the mind: Using space to think about time , 2008, Cognition.

[44]  Laura A. Carlson,et al.  Inhibition within a reference frame during the interpretation of spatial language , 2008, Cognition.

[45]  D. Smilek,et al.  Ovals of time: Time-space associations in synaesthesia , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.

[46]  Anne Maass,et al.  Directional Bias in the Mental Representation of Spatial Events , 2003, Psychological science.

[47]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  SCRIPTS, PLANS, GOALS, AND UNDERSTANDING , 1988 .

[48]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Why the Sunny Side Is Up , 2004, Psychological science.

[49]  K. E. Moore,et al.  Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts , 2006 .

[50]  Bill Jones,et al.  Space-time dependencies in psychophysical judgment of extent and duration: Algebraic models of the tau and kappa effects. , 1982 .

[51]  Julio Santiago,et al.  Time (also) flies from left to right , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[52]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[53]  A. Koriat,et al.  Flexible mental processes in numerical size judgments: The case of hebrew letters that are used to convey numbers , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[54]  I. Nachshon Cross-Cultural Differences in Directionality , 1981 .

[55]  U. Ansorge Spatial Simon effects and compatibility effects induced by observed gaze direction , 2003 .

[56]  Timothy P. McNamara,et al.  Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[57]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[58]  J. Fodor,et al.  The structure of a semantic theory , 1963 .

[59]  Paul Sambre,et al.  Gilles Fauconnier & Mark Turner, " The way we think: conceptual blending and the mind's hidden complexities" , 2002 .

[60]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article THE EFFECT OF IMPLIED ORIENTATION DERIVED FROM VERBAL CONTEXT ON PICTURE RECOGNITION , 2022 .

[61]  L. Boroditsky Does Language Shape Thought?: Mandarin and English Speakers' Conceptions of Time , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[62]  S. Levinson,et al.  Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.3 March 2004 Can language restructure cognition? The case for space , 2022 .

[63]  D. Livesay Right hand left hand , 1977 .

[64]  Wim Fias,et al.  The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized , 2003, Cognition.

[65]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Ad hoc categories , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[66]  J. Pratt,et al.  Symbolic Control of Visual Attention , 2001, Psychological science.

[67]  P. Seymour Internal representation of the months , 1980 .

[68]  Daniel Algom,et al.  Comparative judgment of numerosity and numerical magnitude: attention preempts automaticity. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[69]  M. Studdert-Kennedy Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. , 1994 .

[70]  Rocky Ross,et al.  Mental models , 2004, SIGA.

[71]  R. Jackendoff Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution , 2002 .

[72]  Takeshi Hatta,et al.  Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect , 2004, Memory & cognition.

[73]  Seana Coulson,et al.  Congruity Effects in Time and Space: Behavioral and ERP Measures , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[74]  Barbara Tversky,et al.  Searching Imagined Environments , 1990 .

[75]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Abrupt onsets and gaze direction cues trigger independent reflexive attentional effects , 2003, Cognition.

[76]  E. Hunt,et al.  The Whorfian Hypothesis: A Cognitive Psychology Perspective , 1991 .

[77]  Raymond W. Gibbs,et al.  Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning , 1990, Cognition.

[78]  Michael D. Dodd,et al.  Attentional SNARC: There’s something special about numbers (let us count the ways) , 2008, Cognition.

[79]  Mark Rowlands,et al.  The body in mind , 1999 .

[80]  Thomas M. Spalek,et al.  The Left-to-Right Bias in Inhibition of Return Is Due to the Direction of Reading , 2005, Psychological science.

[81]  Peter Brugger,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[82]  Seyda Özçalskan,et al.  Metaphorical Motion in Crosslinguistic Perspective: A Comparison of English and Turkish , 2003 .

[83]  Anja Ischebeck,et al.  On the relative speed account of number-size interference in comparative judgments of numerals. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[84]  Andrea Tyler,et al.  Reconsidering Prepositional Polysemy Networks: The Case of Over , 2001 .

[85]  John R. Anderson Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .

[86]  H. Helson THE TAU EFFECT--AN EXAMPLE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RELATIVITY. , 1931, Science.

[87]  Rafael E. Núñez,et al.  With the Future Behind Them: Convergent Evidence From Aymara Language and Gesture in the Crosslinguistic Comparison of Spatial Construals of Time , 2006, Cogn. Sci..

[88]  Marc Ouellet,et al.  In hindsight, life flows from left to right , 2010, Psychological research.

[89]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Metaphors We Live By , 1980 .

[90]  L. Pizzamiglio,et al.  When space merges into language , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[91]  Spatial Simon effects with nonspatial responses , 2004 .

[92]  B. Bergen,et al.  The cognitive linguistics enterprise: an overview , 2007 .

[93]  W. Köhler Gestalt psychology , 1967 .

[94]  P. Higham,et al.  The rise and fall of politicians: The judged heights of Broadbent, Mulroney and Turner before and after the 1988 Canadian federal election. , 1992 .

[95]  Eve Sweetser,et al.  From Etymology to Pragmatics: Preface , 1990 .

[96]  L. Boroditsky Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors , 2000, Cognition.

[97]  Øyvind Dahl,et al.  When the future comes from behind: Malagasy and other time concepts and some consequences for communication , 1995 .

[98]  L. Milne,et al.  Right hand, left hand. , 1948, Scientific American.

[99]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Visuospatial priming of the mental number line , 2008, Cognition.

[100]  M. Tops,et al.  Posing for success: Clenching a fist facilitates approach , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[101]  Robert E. Maclaury Semantics and Experience: Universal Metaphors of Time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. HOYT ALVERSON , 1996 .

[102]  B. Shanon Graphological patterns as a function of handedness and culture , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[103]  P. Thagard,et al.  Coherence in Thought and Action , 2000 .

[104]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[105]  Jason Eisner,et al.  Lexical Semantics , 2020, The Handbook of English Linguistics.

[106]  Derek Besner,et al.  Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[107]  Jenn-Yeu Chen,et al.  Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001) , 2007, Cognition.

[108]  Z. Kövecses,et al.  Metaphor in Culture: Metaphor in Culture , 2005 .

[109]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Mapping conceptual to spatial relations in visual reasoning. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[110]  G. Fauconnier,et al.  The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind''s Hidden Complexities. Basic Books , 2002 .

[111]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Embodiment in Attitudes, Social Perception, and Emotion , 2005, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[112]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Things are sounding up: Affective influences on auditory tone perception , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[113]  G. Clore,et al.  Do Approach-Avoidance Actions Create Attitudes? , 2006, Psychological science.

[114]  Mitsuhiko Ota,et al.  Optimality Theory: an overview , 2000 .

[115]  R. Mentzoni,et al.  Where is January? The month-SNARC effect in sequence-form synaesthetes , 2008, Cortex.

[116]  Michael Andres,et al.  Number magnitude potentiates action judgements , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[117]  L. Gleitman,et al.  Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning , 2002, Cognition.

[118]  S. Levinson,et al.  LANGUAGE AND SPACE , 1996 .

[119]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[120]  W. Gevers,et al.  The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line , 2008, Cognition.

[121]  D. Algom,et al.  Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgment of numerals: The role of attention. , 1999 .

[122]  J. Greenstone Relevance , 2007 .

[123]  D. Casasanto,et al.  of Experimental Psychology , 2022 .

[124]  Denise H. Wu,et al.  Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers , 2008, Cognition.

[125]  Samar Zebian,et al.  Linkages between Number Concepts, Spatial Thinking, and Directionality of Writing: The SNARC Effect and the REVERSE SNARC Effect in English and Arabic Monoliterates, Biliterates, and Illiterate Arabic Speakers , 2005 .

[126]  G. Murphy On metaphoric representation , 1996, Cognition.

[127]  A. Fiske The four elementary forms of sociality: framework for a unified theory of social relations. , 1992, Psychological review.

[128]  Giovanni Galfano,et al.  Number magnitude orients attention, but not against one’s will , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[129]  Benjamin A. Motz,et al.  Time After Time: The Psychological Reality of the Ego- and Time-Reference-Point Distinction in Metaphorical Construals of Time , 2006 .

[130]  K. M. Heilman,et al.  Asyntactic Thematic Role Assignment: The Use of a Temporal-Spatial Strategy , 1995, Brain and Language.

[131]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[132]  Marc Ouellet,et al.  Thinking about the future moves attention to the right. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[133]  Robert E. Maclaury,et al.  Zapotec Body-Part Locatives: Prototypes and Metaphoric Extensions , 1989, International Journal of American Linguistics.

[134]  Thomas W. Schubert,et al.  High in the Hierarchy: How Vertical Location and Judgments of Leaders' Power are Interrelated , 2007 .

[135]  B. Sodian,et al.  Preschool children's ability to visually represent relations. , 2008, Developmental science.

[136]  J. Mandler How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. , 1992, Psychological review.

[137]  L. Barsalou Situated simulation in the human conceptual system , 2003 .

[138]  A. Kingstone,et al.  The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze , 1998 .

[139]  Kenneth M Heilman,et al.  Spatial bias: effects of early reading direction on Korean subjects , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[140]  A. Glenberg,et al.  Symbol Grounding and Meaning: A Comparison of High-Dimensional and Embodied Theories of Meaning , 2000 .

[141]  D. Gentner,et al.  Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought , 2003 .

[142]  Wolf Schwarz,et al.  Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[143]  Martin H. Fischer,et al.  Reading space into numbers – a cross-linguistic comparison of the SNARC effect , 2008, Cognition.

[144]  H. Clark,et al.  In cognitive development and the acquisition of language , 1973 .

[145]  D. Algom,et al.  Size congruity effects with two-digit numbers: Expanding the number line? , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[146]  J. Morais,et al.  Spatial associations for musical stimuli: a piano in the head? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[147]  Peter H. Sawchuk,et al.  Perception and emotion , 2010 .

[148]  Samuel Shaki,et al.  Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[149]  Günter Radden,et al.  The Metaphor TIME AS SPACE across Languages , 2003 .

[150]  W. Levelt,et al.  Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .

[151]  Wim Fias,et al.  Shared spatial representations for numbers and space: the reversal of the SNARC and the Simon effects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[152]  John Cohen,et al.  Interdependence of Temporal and Auditory Judgments , 1954, Nature.

[153]  Marie-Pascale Noël,et al.  Images of numbers, or “when 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue” , 1992, Cognition.

[154]  Julio Santiago,et al.  Flexible Conceptual Projection of Time Onto Spatial Frames of Reference , 2006, Cogn. Sci..

[155]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .

[156]  L. Vygotsky,et al.  Thought and Language , 1963 .

[157]  Péter Szigetvári,et al.  What and When? , 2019, Inauguration and Liturgical Kingship in the Long Twelfth Century.

[158]  Kenneth M. Heilman,et al.  Orthographic directionality and thematic role illustration in English and Arabic , 2006, Brain and Language.

[159]  D. Slobin From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking” , 1996 .

[160]  S. Yantis,et al.  Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[161]  M. Phillips,et al.  Questions of tradition , 2004 .

[162]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. , 2002 .

[163]  Christian Dobel,et al.  How Writing System and Age Influence Spatial Representations of Actions , 2007, Psychological science.

[164]  H. H. Clark SPACE, TIME, SEMANTICS, AND THE CHILD , 1973 .

[165]  D. Wentura,et al.  Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of approach- and avoidance-related social information. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[166]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[167]  S. Dreman A review of directionality trends in the horizontal dimension as a function of innate and environmental factors. , 1977, The Journal of general psychology.

[168]  M. McGlone,et al.  Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. , 1998 .

[169]  Ekkehart Malotki,et al.  Hopi Time: A Linguistic Analysis of the Temporal Concepts in the Hopi Language , 1983 .

[170]  Laura A. Carlson-Radvansky,et al.  The Influence of Reference Frame Selection on Spatial Template Construction , 1997 .

[171]  Joan Y. Chiao,et al.  Mental representations of social status , 2004, Cognition.

[172]  Vyvyan Evans,et al.  The cognitive linguistics reader , 2007 .

[173]  A. Kingstone,et al.  The number line effect reflects top-down control , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[174]  L. Talmy Toward a Cognitive Semantics , 2003 .

[175]  S. Levinson Frames of reference and Molyneux's question: Cross-linguistic evidence , 1996 .

[176]  Richard B Ivry,et al.  Temporal Control and Coordination: The Multiple Timer Model , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[177]  F. Thumin,et al.  AUTHORITY STATUS AS A FACTOR IN PERCEPTUAL DISTORTION OF SIZE. , 1964, The Journal of social psychology.

[178]  G. Lakoff,et al.  Metaphors We Live by , 1982 .

[179]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Short Article: Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements Influence Spatial–Numerical Associations , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[180]  D. Kemmerer The spatial and temporal meanings of English prepositions can be independently impaired , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[181]  Andrew R Mayer,et al.  The effects of auditory and visual linguistic distractors on target localization. , 2004, Neuropsychology.

[182]  G. Murphy Reasons to doubt the present evidence for metaphoric representation , 1997, Cognition.

[183]  B. Tversky,et al.  Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[184]  Wim Fias,et al.  Spatial representation of numbers. , 2004 .

[185]  Bruno L. Giordano,et al.  Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect , 2006, Cognition.

[186]  Jan De Houwer Spatial Simon effects with nonspatial responses. , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[187]  K. Heilman,et al.  Agrammatic Sentence Production: The Use of a Temporal-Spatial Strategy , 1995, Brain and Language.

[188]  D. Pecher,et al.  Unconstraining theories of embodied cognition , 2009 .

[189]  D. Casasanto,et al.  Who's Afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic Differences in Temporal Language and Thought , 2008 .

[190]  R. Gibbs,et al.  Embodied experience and linguistic meaning , 2003, Brain and Language.

[191]  J. Förster,et al.  Compatibility between approach/ avoidance stimulation and valenced information determines residual attention during the process of encoding , 2000 .

[192]  D. Mackay The Organization of Perception and Action , 1987 .

[193]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Does "feeling down" mean seeing down? Depressive symptoms and vertical selective attention. , 2006 .

[194]  S. Ian Robertson,et al.  Problem-solving , 2001, Human Thinking.

[195]  Jeanette Altarriba,et al.  Evidence Against Linguistic Relativity in Chinese and English: A Case Study of Spatial and Temporal Metaphors , 2008 .

[196]  Linguistic attention control: attention shifting governed by grammaticized elements of language. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[197]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Language Comprehenders Mentally Represent the Shapes of Objects , 2002, Psychological science.

[198]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Tin Chen, Bargh / Consequences of Automatic Evaluation Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus , 2022 .

[199]  Merideth Leigh Gattis,et al.  Spatial schemas and abstract thought , 2001 .

[200]  Luis Jiménez,et al.  Which Attention Is Needed for Implicit Sequence Learning , 1999 .

[201]  Susan Duncan,et al.  Growth points in thinking-for-speaking , 1998 .

[202]  K. Emmorey Language, Cognition, and the Brain: Insights From Sign Language Research , 2001 .

[203]  A. Wierzbicka,et al.  Semantics and cognition. , 2006, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[204]  Thomas W. Schubert,et al.  Control over the AssoCiAtion of Power And size , 2009 .

[205]  Harriet E. Manelis Klein,et al.  The future precedes the past: Time in Toba , 1987 .

[206]  Kavitha Srinivas,et al.  Orientation Effects in Amnesics' Recognition Memory: Familiarity-Based Access to Object Attributes , 2000 .

[207]  Sarah Florence Taub,et al.  Language from the Body: Iconicity and Metaphor in American Sign Language , 2001 .

[208]  John Cohen,et al.  A New Phenomenon in Time Judgment , 1953, Nature.

[209]  G. Logan Linguistic and Conceptual Control of Visual Spatial Attention , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.

[210]  Übersetzen, interkulturelle Kommunikation, Spracherwerb und Sprachvermittlung - das Leben mit mehreren Sprachen: Festschrift für Juliane House zum 60. Geburtstag. , 2004 .

[211]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Object name Learning Provides On-the-Job Training for Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[212]  Christine D. Wilson,et al.  Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[213]  Hoyt Alverson,et al.  Semantics and Experience: Universal Metaphors of Time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho , 1997 .

[214]  Lera Boroditsky,et al.  The Roles of Body and Mind in Abstract Thought , 2002, Psychological science.

[215]  Vyvyan Evans,et al.  How we conceptualise time : language , meaning and temporal cognition , 2007 .

[216]  B. Repp,et al.  Action Can Affect Auditory Perception , 2007, Psychological science.

[217]  Jay Pratt,et al.  Time flies like an arrow: Space-time compatibility effects suggest the use of a mental timeline , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[218]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention , 2007, Cognition.

[219]  W. Schwarz,et al.  Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: Evidence for a response-related origin , 2005, Memory & cognition.

[220]  M. Chun,et al.  Selective attention modulates implicit learning , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[221]  Drew H. Abney,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Influence of Musical Groove on Postural Sway , 2015 .