Flexible foundations of abstract thought: A review and a theory
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Edward Kako,et al. Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001) , 2007, Cognition.
[2] Jean Piaget,et al. The child's conception of time; , 1969 .
[3] Wim Fias,et al. The Mental Representation of Ordinal Sequences is Spatially Organised: Evidence from Days of the Week , 2004, Cortex.
[4] T. Boult,et al. The eyes have it , 2003, WBMA '03.
[5] Michael D. Robinson,et al. When "light" and "dark" thoughts become light and dark responses: affect biases brightness judgments. , 2007, Emotion.
[6] P. Wilson. Perceptual distortion of height as a function of ascribed academic status. , 1968, The Journal of social psychology.
[7] J. Lammertyn,et al. The hunt for SNARC , 2005 .
[8] Michael D. Dodd,et al. Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[9] Harold Bekkering,et al. Getting a grip on numbers: numerical magnitude priming in object grasping. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] Thomas W. Schubert. Your highness: vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[11] L. Elizabeth Crawford,et al. Affect biases memory of location: Evidence for the spatial representation of affect , 2006 .
[12] A. Markman,et al. Constraining Theories of Embodied Cognition , 2005, Psychological science.
[13] Marina Rakova,et al. The philosophy of embodied realism: A high price to pay? , 2002 .
[14] D. Slobin. Thinking for Speaking , 1987 .
[15] J. Cacioppo,et al. Rudimentary determinants of attitudes. II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[16] S. Glucksberg,et al. Conventional Language: How Metaphorical Is It? , 2000 .
[17] D. Gentner,et al. As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space → time metaphors , 2002 .
[18] David E. Irwin,et al. Frames of reference in vision and language: Where is above? , 1993, Cognition.
[19] L. Barsalou,et al. Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[20] John J. McCarthy,et al. Optimality Theory: An overview , 2003 .
[21] Brian F. Bowdle,et al. The career of metaphor. , 2005, Psychological review.
[22] P. Johnson-Laird,et al. Preferred mental models in reasoning about spatial relations , 2007, Memory & cognition.
[23] Rolf A. Zwaan. The Immersed Experiencer: Toward An Embodied Theory Of Language Comprehension , 2003 .
[24] Sarah F. Taub. Iconicity in American sign language: concrete and metaphorical applications , 2000, Spatial Cogn. Comput..
[25] Irvin Rock,et al. Orientation and form , 1974 .
[26] B. Bergum,et al. Attention and performance IX , 1982 .
[27] Antonino Vallesi,et al. An effect of spatial–temporal association of response codes: Understanding the cognitive representations of time , 2008, Cognition.
[28] David H. Silvera,et al. Judgment by Quantity , 1994 .
[29] W. Bechtel,et al. A companion to cognitive science , 1999 .
[30] Alan Garnham. A unified theory of the meaning of some spatial relational terms , 1989, Cognition.
[31] S. Levinson,et al. Returning the tables: language affects spatial reasoning , 2002, Cognition.
[32] N. Sagiv,et al. What is the relationship between synaesthesia and visuo-spatial number forms? , 2006, Cognition.
[33] Effects of stimulus–stimulus short-term memory associations in a Simon-like task , 2008 .
[34] R. Levine,et al. A Geography Of Time , 1997 .
[35] A. Henik,et al. Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks , 1982, Memory & cognition.
[36] B. Landau,et al. “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition , 1993 .
[37] Gordon D. Logan,et al. A computational analysis of the apprehension of spatial relations , 1996 .
[38] D. E. Irwin,et al. Reference Frame Activation during Spatial Term Assignment , 1994 .
[39] G. Lakoff,et al. Philosophy in the flesh : the embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought , 1999 .
[40] N. Humphrey,et al. Turning the Left Cheek , 1973, Nature.
[41] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[42] A. Chatterjee,et al. Verbs, events and spatial representations , 1999, Neuropsychologia.
[43] L. Boroditsky,et al. Time in the mind: Using space to think about time , 2008, Cognition.
[44] Laura A. Carlson,et al. Inhibition within a reference frame during the interpretation of spatial language , 2008, Cognition.
[45] D. Smilek,et al. Ovals of time: Time-space associations in synaesthesia , 2007, Consciousness and Cognition.
[46] Anne Maass,et al. Directional Bias in the Mental Representation of Spatial Events , 2003, Psychological science.
[47] Roger C. Schank,et al. SCRIPTS, PLANS, GOALS, AND UNDERSTANDING , 1988 .
[48] Michael D. Robinson,et al. Why the Sunny Side Is Up , 2004, Psychological science.
[49] K. E. Moore,et al. Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts , 2006 .
[50] Bill Jones,et al. Space-time dependencies in psychophysical judgment of extent and duration: Algebraic models of the tau and kappa effects. , 1982 .
[51] Julio Santiago,et al. Time (also) flies from left to right , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[52] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[53] A. Koriat,et al. Flexible mental processes in numerical size judgments: The case of hebrew letters that are used to convey numbers , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[54] I. Nachshon. Cross-Cultural Differences in Directionality , 1981 .
[55] U. Ansorge. Spatial Simon effects and compatibility effects induced by observed gaze direction , 2003 .
[56] Timothy P. McNamara,et al. Systems of Spatial Reference in Human Memory , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.
[57] Daniel C. Richardson,et al. Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs , 2003, Cogn. Sci..
[58] J. Fodor,et al. The structure of a semantic theory , 1963 .
[59] Paul Sambre,et al. Gilles Fauconnier & Mark Turner, " The way we think: conceptual blending and the mind's hidden complexities" , 2002 .
[60] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article THE EFFECT OF IMPLIED ORIENTATION DERIVED FROM VERBAL CONTEXT ON PICTURE RECOGNITION , 2022 .
[61] L. Boroditsky. Does Language Shape Thought?: Mandarin and English Speakers' Conceptions of Time , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.
[62] S. Levinson,et al. Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.8 No.3 March 2004 Can language restructure cognition? The case for space , 2022 .
[63] D. Livesay. Right hand left hand , 1977 .
[64] Wim Fias,et al. The mental representation of ordinal sequences is spatially organized , 2003, Cognition.
[65] L. Barsalou,et al. Ad hoc categories , 1983, Memory & cognition.
[66] J. Pratt,et al. Symbolic Control of Visual Attention , 2001, Psychological science.
[67] P. Seymour. Internal representation of the months , 1980 .
[68] Daniel Algom,et al. Comparative judgment of numerosity and numerical magnitude: attention preempts automaticity. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[69] M. Studdert-Kennedy. Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. , 1994 .
[70] Rocky Ross,et al. Mental models , 2004, SIGA.
[71] R. Jackendoff. Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution , 2002 .
[72] Takeshi Hatta,et al. Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect , 2004, Memory & cognition.
[73] Seana Coulson,et al. Congruity Effects in Time and Space: Behavioral and ERP Measures , 2008, Cogn. Sci..
[74] Barbara Tversky,et al. Searching Imagined Environments , 1990 .
[75] Alan Kingstone,et al. Abrupt onsets and gaze direction cues trigger independent reflexive attentional effects , 2003, Cognition.
[76] E. Hunt,et al. The Whorfian Hypothesis: A Cognitive Psychology Perspective , 1991 .
[77] Raymond W. Gibbs,et al. Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning , 1990, Cognition.
[78] Michael D. Dodd,et al. Attentional SNARC: There’s something special about numbers (let us count the ways) , 2008, Cognition.
[79] Mark Rowlands,et al. The body in mind , 1999 .
[80] Thomas M. Spalek,et al. The Left-to-Right Bias in Inhibition of Return Is Due to the Direction of Reading , 2005, Psychological science.
[81] Peter Brugger,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[82] Seyda Özçalskan,et al. Metaphorical Motion in Crosslinguistic Perspective: A Comparison of English and Turkish , 2003 .
[83] Anja Ischebeck,et al. On the relative speed account of number-size interference in comparative judgments of numerals. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[84] Andrea Tyler,et al. Reconsidering Prepositional Polysemy Networks: The Case of Over , 2001 .
[85] John R. Anderson. Acquisition of cognitive skill. , 1982 .
[86] H. Helson. THE TAU EFFECT--AN EXAMPLE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RELATIVITY. , 1931, Science.
[87] Rafael E. Núñez,et al. With the Future Behind Them: Convergent Evidence From Aymara Language and Gesture in the Crosslinguistic Comparison of Spatial Construals of Time , 2006, Cogn. Sci..
[88] Marc Ouellet,et al. In hindsight, life flows from left to right , 2010, Psychological research.
[89] G. Lakoff,et al. Metaphors We Live By , 1980 .
[90] L. Pizzamiglio,et al. When space merges into language , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[91] Spatial Simon effects with nonspatial responses , 2004 .
[92] B. Bergen,et al. The cognitive linguistics enterprise: an overview , 2007 .
[93] W. Köhler. Gestalt psychology , 1967 .
[94] P. Higham,et al. The rise and fall of politicians: The judged heights of Broadbent, Mulroney and Turner before and after the 1988 Canadian federal election. , 1992 .
[95] Eve Sweetser,et al. From Etymology to Pragmatics: Preface , 1990 .
[96] L. Boroditsky. Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors , 2000, Cognition.
[97] Øyvind Dahl,et al. When the future comes from behind: Malagasy and other time concepts and some consequences for communication , 1995 .
[98] L. Milne,et al. Right hand, left hand. , 1948, Scientific American.
[99] Marco Zorzi,et al. Visuospatial priming of the mental number line , 2008, Cognition.
[100] M. Tops,et al. Posing for success: Clenching a fist facilitates approach , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[101] Robert E. Maclaury. Semantics and Experience: Universal Metaphors of Time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. HOYT ALVERSON , 1996 .
[102] B. Shanon. Graphological patterns as a function of handedness and culture , 1979, Neuropsychologia.
[103] P. Thagard,et al. Coherence in Thought and Action , 2000 .
[104] Michael P. Kaschak,et al. Grounding language in action , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[105] Jason Eisner,et al. Lexical Semantics , 2020, The Handbook of English Linguistics.
[106] Derek Besner,et al. Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English , 1979, Neuropsychologia.
[107] Jenn-Yeu Chen,et al. Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001) , 2007, Cognition.
[108] Z. Kövecses,et al. Metaphor in Culture: Metaphor in Culture , 2005 .
[109] K. Holyoak,et al. Mapping conceptual to spatial relations in visual reasoning. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[110] G. Fauconnier,et al. The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind''s Hidden Complexities. Basic Books , 2002 .
[111] L. Barsalou,et al. Embodiment in Attitudes, Social Perception, and Emotion , 2005, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.
[112] Michael D. Robinson,et al. Things are sounding up: Affective influences on auditory tone perception , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[113] G. Clore,et al. Do Approach-Avoidance Actions Create Attitudes? , 2006, Psychological science.
[114] Mitsuhiko Ota,et al. Optimality Theory: an overview , 2000 .
[115] R. Mentzoni,et al. Where is January? The month-SNARC effect in sequence-form synaesthetes , 2008, Cortex.
[116] Michael Andres,et al. Number magnitude potentiates action judgements , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[117] L. Gleitman,et al. Turning the tables: language and spatial reasoning , 2002, Cognition.
[118] S. Levinson,et al. LANGUAGE AND SPACE , 1996 .
[119] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[120] W. Gevers,et al. The SNARC effect does not imply a mental number line , 2008, Cognition.
[121] D. Algom,et al. Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgment of numerals: The role of attention. , 1999 .
[122] J. Greenstone. Relevance , 2007 .
[123] D. Casasanto,et al. of Experimental Psychology , 2022 .
[124] Denise H. Wu,et al. Flexible spatial mapping of different notations of numbers in Chinese readers , 2008, Cognition.
[125] Samar Zebian,et al. Linkages between Number Concepts, Spatial Thinking, and Directionality of Writing: The SNARC Effect and the REVERSE SNARC Effect in English and Arabic Monoliterates, Biliterates, and Illiterate Arabic Speakers , 2005 .
[126] G. Murphy. On metaphoric representation , 1996, Cognition.
[127] A. Fiske. The four elementary forms of sociality: framework for a unified theory of social relations. , 1992, Psychological review.
[128] Giovanni Galfano,et al. Number magnitude orients attention, but not against one’s will , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[129] Benjamin A. Motz,et al. Time After Time: The Psychological Reality of the Ego- and Time-Reference-Point Distinction in Metaphorical Construals of Time , 2006 .
[130] K. M. Heilman,et al. Asyntactic Thematic Role Assignment: The Use of a Temporal-Spatial Strategy , 1995, Brain and Language.
[131] S. Dehaene,et al. Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[132] Marc Ouellet,et al. Thinking about the future moves attention to the right. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[133] Robert E. Maclaury,et al. Zapotec Body-Part Locatives: Prototypes and Metaphoric Extensions , 1989, International Journal of American Linguistics.
[134] Thomas W. Schubert,et al. High in the Hierarchy: How Vertical Location and Judgments of Leaders' Power are Interrelated , 2007 .
[135] B. Sodian,et al. Preschool children's ability to visually represent relations. , 2008, Developmental science.
[136] J. Mandler. How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. , 1992, Psychological review.
[137] L. Barsalou. Situated simulation in the human conceptual system , 2003 .
[138] A. Kingstone,et al. The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze , 1998 .
[139] Kenneth M Heilman,et al. Spatial bias: effects of early reading direction on Korean subjects , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[140] A. Glenberg,et al. Symbol Grounding and Meaning: A Comparison of High-Dimensional and Embodied Theories of Meaning , 2000 .
[141] D. Gentner,et al. Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought , 2003 .
[142] Wolf Schwarz,et al. Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[143] Martin H. Fischer,et al. Reading space into numbers – a cross-linguistic comparison of the SNARC effect , 2008, Cognition.
[144] H. Clark,et al. In cognitive development and the acquisition of language , 1973 .
[145] D. Algom,et al. Size congruity effects with two-digit numbers: Expanding the number line? , 2006, Memory & cognition.
[146] J. Morais,et al. Spatial associations for musical stimuli: a piano in the head? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[147] Peter H. Sawchuk,et al. Perception and emotion , 2010 .
[148] Samuel Shaki,et al. Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[149] Günter Radden,et al. The Metaphor TIME AS SPACE across Languages , 2003 .
[150] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[151] Wim Fias,et al. Shared spatial representations for numbers and space: the reversal of the SNARC and the Simon effects. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[152] John Cohen,et al. Interdependence of Temporal and Auditory Judgments , 1954, Nature.
[153] Marie-Pascale Noël,et al. Images of numbers, or “when 98 is upper left and 6 sky blue” , 1992, Cognition.
[154] Julio Santiago,et al. Flexible Conceptual Projection of Time Onto Spatial Frames of Reference , 2006, Cogn. Sci..
[155] Linda B. Smith,et al. The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .
[156] L. Vygotsky,et al. Thought and Language , 1963 .
[157] Péter Szigetvári,et al. What and When? , 2019, Inauguration and Liturgical Kingship in the Long Twelfth Century.
[158] Kenneth M. Heilman,et al. Orthographic directionality and thematic role illustration in English and Arabic , 2006, Brain and Language.
[159] D. Slobin. From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking” , 1996 .
[160] S. Yantis,et al. Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.
[161] M. Phillips,et al. Questions of tradition , 2004 .
[162] J. Lupiáñez,et al. A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. , 2002 .
[163] Christian Dobel,et al. How Writing System and Age Influence Spatial Representations of Actions , 2007, Psychological science.
[164] H. H. Clark. SPACE, TIME, SEMANTICS, AND THE CHILD , 1973 .
[165] D. Wentura,et al. Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of approach- and avoidance-related social information. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[166] Siobhan Chapman. Logic and Conversation , 2005 .
[167] S. Dreman. A review of directionality trends in the horizontal dimension as a function of innate and environmental factors. , 1977, The Journal of general psychology.
[168] M. McGlone,et al. Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. , 1998 .
[169] Ekkehart Malotki,et al. Hopi Time: A Linguistic Analysis of the Temporal Concepts in the Hopi Language , 1983 .
[170] Laura A. Carlson-Radvansky,et al. The Influence of Reference Frame Selection on Spatial Template Construction , 1997 .
[171] Joan Y. Chiao,et al. Mental representations of social status , 2004, Cognition.
[172] Vyvyan Evans,et al. The cognitive linguistics reader , 2007 .
[173] A. Kingstone,et al. The number line effect reflects top-down control , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[174] L. Talmy. Toward a Cognitive Semantics , 2003 .
[175] S. Levinson. Frames of reference and Molyneux's question: Cross-linguistic evidence , 1996 .
[176] Richard B Ivry,et al. Temporal Control and Coordination: The Multiple Timer Model , 2002, Brain and Cognition.
[177] F. Thumin,et al. AUTHORITY STATUS AS A FACTOR IN PERCEPTUAL DISTORTION OF SIZE. , 1964, The Journal of social psychology.
[178] G. Lakoff,et al. Metaphors We Live by , 1982 .
[179] H. Bekkering,et al. Short Article: Coding Strategies in Number Space: Memory Requirements Influence Spatial–Numerical Associations , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[180] D. Kemmerer. The spatial and temporal meanings of English prepositions can be independently impaired , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[181] Andrew R Mayer,et al. The effects of auditory and visual linguistic distractors on target localization. , 2004, Neuropsychology.
[182] G. Murphy. Reasons to doubt the present evidence for metaphoric representation , 1997, Cognition.
[183] B. Tversky,et al. Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.
[184] Wim Fias,et al. Spatial representation of numbers. , 2004 .
[185] Bruno L. Giordano,et al. Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect , 2006, Cognition.
[186] Jan De Houwer. Spatial Simon effects with nonspatial responses. , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[187] K. Heilman,et al. Agrammatic Sentence Production: The Use of a Temporal-Spatial Strategy , 1995, Brain and Language.
[188] D. Pecher,et al. Unconstraining theories of embodied cognition , 2009 .
[189] D. Casasanto,et al. Who's Afraid of the Big Bad Whorf? Crosslinguistic Differences in Temporal Language and Thought , 2008 .
[190] R. Gibbs,et al. Embodied experience and linguistic meaning , 2003, Brain and Language.
[191] J. Förster,et al. Compatibility between approach/ avoidance stimulation and valenced information determines residual attention during the process of encoding , 2000 .
[192] D. Mackay. The Organization of Perception and Action , 1987 .
[193] Michael D. Robinson,et al. Does "feeling down" mean seeing down? Depressive symptoms and vertical selective attention. , 2006 .
[194] S. Ian Robertson,et al. Problem-solving , 2001, Human Thinking.
[195] Jeanette Altarriba,et al. Evidence Against Linguistic Relativity in Chinese and English: A Case Study of Spatial and Temporal Metaphors , 2008 .
[196] Linguistic attention control: attention shifting governed by grammaticized elements of language. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[197] Rolf A. Zwaan,et al. Language Comprehenders Mentally Represent the Shapes of Objects , 2002, Psychological science.
[198] S. Chaiken,et al. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Tin Chen, Bargh / Consequences of Automatic Evaluation Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus , 2022 .
[199] Merideth Leigh Gattis,et al. Spatial schemas and abstract thought , 2001 .
[200] Luis Jiménez,et al. Which Attention Is Needed for Implicit Sequence Learning , 1999 .
[201] Susan Duncan,et al. Growth points in thinking-for-speaking , 1998 .
[202] K. Emmorey. Language, Cognition, and the Brain: Insights From Sign Language Research , 2001 .
[203] A. Wierzbicka,et al. Semantics and cognition. , 2006, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.
[204] Thomas W. Schubert,et al. Control over the AssoCiAtion of Power And size , 2009 .
[205] Harriet E. Manelis Klein,et al. The future precedes the past: Time in Toba , 1987 .
[206] Kavitha Srinivas,et al. Orientation Effects in Amnesics' Recognition Memory: Familiarity-Based Access to Object Attributes , 2000 .
[207] Sarah Florence Taub,et al. Language from the Body: Iconicity and Metaphor in American Sign Language , 2001 .
[208] John Cohen,et al. A New Phenomenon in Time Judgment , 1953, Nature.
[209] G. Logan. Linguistic and Conceptual Control of Visual Spatial Attention , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.
[211] Linda B. Smith,et al. Object name Learning Provides On-the-Job Training for Attention , 2002, Psychological science.
[212] Christine D. Wilson,et al. Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[213] Hoyt Alverson,et al. Semantics and Experience: Universal Metaphors of Time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho , 1997 .
[214] Lera Boroditsky,et al. The Roles of Body and Mind in Abstract Thought , 2002, Psychological science.
[215] Vyvyan Evans,et al. How we conceptualise time : language , meaning and temporal cognition , 2007 .
[216] B. Repp,et al. Action Can Affect Auditory Perception , 2007, Psychological science.
[217] Jay Pratt,et al. Time flies like an arrow: Space-time compatibility effects suggest the use of a mental timeline , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[218] Marco Zorzi,et al. Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention , 2007, Cognition.
[219] W. Schwarz,et al. Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect: Evidence for a response-related origin , 2005, Memory & cognition.
[220] M. Chun,et al. Selective attention modulates implicit learning , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[221] Drew H. Abney,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Influence of Musical Groove on Postural Sway , 2015 .