Adaptive Social Learning Strategies in Temporally and Spatially Varying Environments

Long before the origins of agriculture human ancestors had expanded across the globe into an immense variety of environments, from Australian deserts to Siberian tundra. Survival in these environments did not principally depend on genetic adaptations, but instead on evolved learning strategies that permitted the assembly of locally adaptive behavioral repertoires. To develop hypotheses about these learning strategies, we have modeled the evolution of learning strategies to assess what conditions and constraints favor which kinds of strategies. To build on prior work, we focus on clarifying how spatial variability, temporal variability, and the number of cultural traits influence the evolution of four types of strategies: (1) individual learning, (2) unbiased social learning, (3) payoff-biased social learning, and (4) conformist transmission. Using a combination of analytic and simulation methods, we show that spatial—but not temporal—variation strongly favors the emergence of conformist transmission. This effect intensifies when migration rates are relatively high and individual learning is costly. We also show that increasing the number of cultural traits above two favors the evolution of conformist transmission, which suggests that the assumption of only two traits in many models has been conservative. We close by discussing how (1) spatial variability represents only one way of introducing the low-level, nonadaptive phenotypic trait variation that so favors conformist transmission, the other obvious way being learning errors, and (2) our findings apply to the evolution of conformist transmission in social interactions. Throughout we emphasize how our models generate empirical predictions suitable for laboratory testing.

[1]  K. Laland Social learning strategies , 2004, Learning & behavior.

[2]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. , 2001, Evolution and human behavior : official journal of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society.

[3]  R. Boyd,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. , 1998 .

[4]  Joe Yuichiro Wakano,et al.  A mixed strategy model for the emergence and intensification of social learning in a periodically changing natural environment. , 2006, Theoretical population biology.

[5]  Mark Lubell,et al.  Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission , 2008 .

[6]  P. Richerson,et al.  The Evolution of Ethnic Markers , 1987 .

[7]  Hal Whitehead,et al.  The evolution of conformist social learning can cause population collapse in realistically variable environments , 2009 .

[8]  R. Boyd,et al.  Coordinated Punishment of Defectors Sustains Cooperation and Can Proliferate When Rare , 2010, Science.

[9]  R Boyd,et al.  Why people punish defectors. Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. , 2001, Journal of theoretical biology.

[10]  Richard McElreath,et al.  Dual-inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution , 2007 .

[11]  Pontus Strimling,et al.  When Natural Selection Favors Imitation of Parents , 2008, Current Anthropology.

[12]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  On the nature of cultural transmission networks: evidence from Fijian villages for adaptive learning biases , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  M. Gadgil,et al.  Adaptive significance of the Indian caste system: an ecological perspective. , 1983, Annals of human biology.

[14]  K. Schlag Why Imitate, and If So, How?, : A Boundedly Rational Approach to Multi-armed Bandits , 1998 .

[15]  Richard McElreath,et al.  Culture rather than genes provides greater scope for the evolution of large-scale human prosociality , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Prestige-biased cultural learning: bystander's differential attention to potential models influences children's learning , 2012 .

[17]  Kathleen H Corriveau,et al.  Young children's selective trust in informants , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[18]  B. Galef,et al.  Use of social information by sodium- and protein-deficient rats: test of a prediction (Boyd & Richerson 1988) , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  J. Henrich Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation , 2004 .

[20]  Kevin N Laland,et al.  An investigation of the relationship between innovation and cultural diversity. , 2009, Theoretical population biology.

[21]  T. Mexia,et al.  Author ' s personal copy , 2009 .

[22]  T Takahashi,et al.  [Cultural anthropology. 2]. , 1968, [Kango kyoiku] Japanese journal of nurses' education.

[23]  Magnus Enquist,et al.  CRITICAL POINTS IN CURRENT THEORY OF CONFORMIST SOCIAL LEARNING , 2007 .

[24]  R. Boyd,et al.  The evolution of altruistic punishment , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  Hal Whitehead,et al.  Learning, climate and the evolution of cultural capacity. , 2007, Journal of theoretical biology.

[26]  Allon J. Uhlmann Coevolution: genes, culture and human diversity , 1993 .

[27]  Wataru Nakahashi,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission in social learning when the environment changes periodically. , 2007, Theoretical population biology.

[28]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  Division of Labor, Economic Specialization, and the Evolution of Social Stratification , 2008, Current Anthropology.

[29]  Peter Hammerstein,et al.  Game theory and human evolution: a critique of some recent interpretations of experimental games. , 2006, Theoretical population biology.

[30]  Daisuke Nakanishi,et al.  Cost–benefit analysis of social/cultural learning in a nonstationary uncertain environment: An evolutionary simulation and an experiment with human subjects , 2002 .

[31]  Joe Yuichiro Wakano,et al.  Do social learning and conformist bias coevolve? Henrich and Boyd revisited. , 2007, Theoretical population biology.

[32]  R. Boyd,et al.  On Modeling Cognition and Culture: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations , 2002 .

[33]  R. Mcelreath,et al.  Shared Norms and the Evolution of Ethnic Markers , 2003, Current Anthropology.

[34]  John Allen A Guttman Scale Analysis of Matsigenka Men's Manufacturing Skills , 1995 .

[35]  中橋 渉,et al.  The evolution of conformist transmission in social learning when the environment fluctuates , 2007 .

[36]  Ruth Mace,et al.  Political complexity predicts the spread of ethnolinguistic groups , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  Murray L. Wax,et al.  Sick Societies: Challenging the Myth of Primitive Harmony , 1994 .

[38]  E. Fehr,et al.  The Coevolution of Cultural Groups and Ingroup Favoritism , 2008, Science.

[39]  R. Boyd,et al.  Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem , 2004, Nature.

[40]  J. Henrich,et al.  The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[41]  Kenichi Aoki,et al.  EVOLUTION OF THE SOCIAL‐LEARNER‐EXPLORER STRATEGY IN AN ENVIRONMENTALLY HETEROGENEOUS TWO‐ISLAND MODEL , 2010, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.

[42]  Robert Boyd,et al.  Rapid cultural adaptation can facilitate the evolution of large-scale cooperation , 2010, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[43]  K. Laland,et al.  Cognitive culture: theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  Alex Mesoudi,et al.  How cultural evolutionary theory can inform social psychology and vice versa. , 2009, Psychological review.

[45]  Morton H. Fried,et al.  The Evolution of Political Society: An Essay in Political Anthropology , 1967 .

[46]  Kenichi Aoki,et al.  Evolution of learning in subdivided populations that occupy environmentally heterogeneous sites. , 2008, Theoretical population biology.

[47]  W. Hoppitt,et al.  Is all learning innovation? , 2007, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[48]  Mark Lubell,et al.  Beyond existence and aiming outside the laboratory: estimating frequency-dependent and pay-off-biased social learning strategies , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[49]  Luc-Alain Giraldeau,et al.  The evolution of social learning rules: payoff-biased and frequency-dependent biased transmission. , 2009, Journal of theoretical biology.

[50]  K. Laland,et al.  From fish to fashion: experimental and theoretical insights into the evolution of culture , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[51]  Robert Rowthorn,et al.  When in Rome, do as the Romans do: the coevolution of altruistic punishment, conformist learning, and cooperation , 2007 .

[52]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology , 2007 .

[53]  J. Henrich,et al.  Why Humans Cooperate: A Cultural and Evolutionary Explanation , 2007 .

[54]  Joe Yuichiro Wakano,et al.  Evolution of social learning: a mathematical analysis. , 2004, Theoretical population biology.

[55]  M. Feldman,et al.  Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. , 1981, Monographs in population biology.

[56]  Gunilla Stenberg,et al.  Selectivity in Infant Social Referencing. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[57]  Karl H. Schlag,et al.  Which One Should I Imitate , 1999 .

[58]  Stephen Shennan,et al.  Branching, blending, and the evolution of cultural similarities and differences among human populations , 2006 .

[59]  F. Barth,et al.  Political Leadership Among Swat Pathans , 2020 .

[60]  H. Gintis,et al.  Costly signaling and cooperation. , 2001, Journal of theoretical biology.