Short- and long-term plasticity of eye position information: examining perceptual, attentional, and motor influences on perisaccadic perception.

Spatial vision requires information about eye position to account for eye movements. But integrating eye position information and information about objects in the world is imperfect and can lead to transient misperceptions around the time of saccadic eye movements most likely because the signals are prone to temporal errors making it difficult to tell when the retinas move relative to when retinal images move. To clarify where this uncertainty comes from, in four experiments we examined influences of eye posture, attentional cueing, and trial history on perisaccadic misperceptions. We found evidence for one longer-term modulation of perisaccadic shift that evolved over the time of the test session due to biased eye posture. Another, short-term influence on perisaccadic shift was related to eye posture during preceding trials or the direction of the preceding saccade. Both perceptual effects could not be explained with visual delays, influences of attention or changes in saccade metrics. Our data are consistent with the idea that perisaccadic shift is caused by neural representations of eye position or space that are plastic and that arise from non-motor, extraretinal mechanisms. This suggests a perceptual system that continuously calibrates itself in response to changes in oculomotor and muscle systems to reconstruct a stable percept of the world.

[1]  Jordan Pola,et al.  Models of the mechanism underlying perceived location of a perisaccadic flash , 2004, Vision Research.

[2]  L. Matin,et al.  Saccadic suppression of displacement: Separate influences of saccade size and of target retinal eccentricity , 1997, Vision Research.

[3]  J. Schlag,et al.  Illusory localization of stimuli flashed in the dark before saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[4]  Masaki Tanaka Spatiotemporal properties of eye position signals in the primate central thalamus. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  Junying Yuan,et al.  Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex , 2022 .

[6]  J. Douglas Crawford,et al.  Optimal inference explains dimension-specific contractions of spatial perception , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Exploring the consequences of the previous trial , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  P Dassonville,et al.  Oculomotor localization relies on a damped representation of saccadic eye displacement in human and nonhuman primates , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[9]  D. Munoz,et al.  t Immediate Neural Plasticity Shapes Motor Performance , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  David C Burr,et al.  Fusion of Visual and Auditory Stimuli during Saccades: A Bayesian Explanation for Perisaccadic Distortions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[13]  Bart Krekelberg,et al.  Postsaccadic visual references generate presaccadic compression of space , 2000, Nature.

[14]  E Kowler,et al.  Illusory shifts in visual direction accompany adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 1999, Nature.

[15]  R. Wurtz,et al.  A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.

[16]  M. Lappe,et al.  Contrast dependency of saccadic compression and suppression , 2004, Vision Research.

[17]  J. Lynch,et al.  Cortico-cortical networks and cortico-subcortical loops for the higher control of eye movements. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[18]  M. Concetta Morrone,et al.  Apparent Position of Visual Targets during Real and Simulated Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Markus Lappe,et al.  The Peri-Saccadic Perception of Objects and Space , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[20]  H. H. Chung,et al.  Dynamic representation of eye position in the parieto-occipital sulcus. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[22]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.

[23]  J. D. Connolly,et al.  fMRI activation in the human frontal eye field is correlated with saccadic reaction time. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.

[25]  M. Perenin,et al.  Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect , 1998, Nature.

[26]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Frontal eye field activity before visual search errors reveals the integration of bottom-up and top-down salience. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  D A Robinson,et al.  Hysteresis and slow drift in abducens unit activity. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  L. Matin,et al.  Visual Perception of Direction for Stimuli Flashed During Voluntary Saccadic Eye Movements , 1965, Science.

[29]  H. Honda The time courses of visual mislocalization and of extraretinal eye position signals at the time of vertical saccades , 1991, Vision Research.

[30]  N. Bischof,et al.  Untersuchungen und Überlegungen zur Richtungswahrnehmung bei willkürlichen sakkadischen Augenbewegungen , 1968 .

[31]  F. Hamker The reentry hypothesis: the putative interaction of the frontal eye field, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and areas V4, IT for attention and eye movement. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  Daniel C. Javitt,et al.  Right hemisphere control of visuospatial attention: line-bisection judgments evaluated with high-density electrical mapping and source analysis☆ , 2003, NeuroImage.

[33]  K R Paap,et al.  Perceptual consequences of potentiation in the extraocular muscles: an alternative explanation for adaptation to wedge prisms. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Human fMRI evidence for the neural correlates of preparatory set , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  L. Stark,et al.  Ocular proprioception and efference copy in registering visual direction , 1991, Vision Research.

[36]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Mislocalization of Perceived Saccade Target Position Induced by Perisaccadic Visual Stimulation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Christopher J Bockisch,et al.  Different motor systems use similar damped extraretinal eye position information , 1999, Vision Research.

[38]  A. V. van Opstal,et al.  Perisaccadic mislocalization of visual targets by head-free gaze shifts: visual or motor? , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  N. Bischof,et al.  [Investigations and considerations of directional perception during voluntary saccadic eye movements]. , 1968, Psychologische Forschung.

[40]  Carsten Finke,et al.  Perisaccadic Compression Correlates with Saccadic Peak Velocity: Differential Association of Eye Movement Dynamics with Perceptual Mislocalization Patterns , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Afferent delays and the mislocalization of perisaccadic stimuli , 2001, Vision Research.

[42]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[43]  J Schlag,et al.  Visuomotor functions of central thalamus in monkey. I. Unit activity related to spontaneous eye movements. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Susumu Tachi,et al.  Perisaccadic perception of continuous flickers , 2005, Vision Research.

[45]  John N. Park Displacement of Apparent Straight Ahead as an Aftereffect of Deviation of the Eyes from Normal Position , 1969, Perceptual and motor skills.

[46]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[47]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Neuronal Basis of Covert Spatial Attention in the Frontal Eye Field , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  D. Sparks,et al.  Corollary discharge provides accurate eye position information to the oculomotor system. , 1983, Science.

[49]  Repetition priming reveals sustained facilitation and transient inhibition in reaction time. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[50]  J. Douglas Crawford,et al.  Optimal transsaccadic integration explains distorted spatial perception , 2003, Nature.

[51]  D. Munoz,et al.  Saccadic Probability Influences Motor Preparation Signals and Time to Saccadic Initiation , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The role of location probability in the programming of saccades: Implications for “center-of-gravity” tendencies , 1989, Vision Research.

[53]  M. Lappe,et al.  Perception of visual space at the time of pro- and anti-saccades. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.