Ocular following responses of the marmoset monkey are dependent on post-saccadic delay, spatiotemporal frequency and saccade direction

Ocular following is a short-latency, reflexive eye movement that tracks wide-field visual motion. It has been studied extensively in humans and macaques and is an appealing behaviour for studying sensory-motor transformations in the brain due to its rapidity and rigidity. We explored ocular following in the marmoset, an emerging model in neuroscience because their lissencephalic brain allows direct access to most cortical areas for imaging and electrophysiological recordings. In three experiments, we tested ocular following responses in three adult marmosets. First, we varied the gap between saccade end and stimulus motion onset (post-saccadic delay), from 10 to 300 ms. As in other species, tracking had shorter onset latencies and higher eye speeds with shorter post-saccadic delays. Second, using sine-wave grating stimuli we explored the dependence of eye speed on spatiotemporal frequency. The highest eye speed was evoked at ∼16 Hz and ∼0.16 cycles per degree (cpd), however, the highest gain was elicited at ∼1.6 Hz and ∼1.2 cpd. The highest eye speed for each spatial frequency was observed at a different temporal frequency, but this interdependence was not consistent with complete speed tuning of the ocular following response. Finally, we found the highest eye speeds when saccade and stimulus motion directions were congruent, although latencies were unaffected by direction congruence. Our results showed comparable ocular following in marmosets, humans and macaques, despite over an order of magnitude variation in body and eye size across species. This characterization will help future studies examining the neural basis of sensory-motor transformations. New & Noteworthy Previous ocular following studies focused on humans and macaques. We examined the properties of ocular following responses in marmosets in three experiments, in which post-saccadic delay, spatial-temporal frequency of stimuli and congruency of saccade and motion directions were manipulated. We have demonstrated short-latency ocular following in marmosets, and discuss the similarities across three species that vary markedly in eye and head size. Our findings will help future studies examining the neural mechanism of sensory-motor transformations.

[1]  A. Souto,et al.  Active vision during prey capture in wild marmoset monkeys , 2022, Current Biology.

[2]  Nicholas S. C. Price,et al.  Understanding Sensory Information Processing Through Simultaneous Multi-area Population Recordings , 2019, Front. Neural Circuits.

[3]  Alexander C. Huk,et al.  Lawful tracking of visual motion in humans, macaques, and marmosets in a naturalistic, continuous, and untrained behavioral context , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Ruth E. Hogg,et al.  Individual differences in human eye movements: An oculomotor signature? , 2017, Vision Research.

[5]  Cory T. Miller,et al.  Motion dependence of smooth pursuit eye movements in the marmoset. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Leo L. Lui,et al.  Structure and function of the middle temporal visual area (MT) in the marmoset: Comparisons with the macaque monkey , 2015, Neuroscience Research.

[7]  Nicholas S C Price,et al.  Reflexive tracking eye movements and motion perception: one or two neural populations? , 2014, Journal of vision.

[8]  K. Kawano,et al.  Difference in Visual Motion Representation between Cortical Areas MT and MST during Ocular Following Responses , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Kenji Kawano,et al.  Initiation of the optokinetic response (OKR) in mice. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[10]  Guillaume S Masson,et al.  Temporal dynamics of 2D motion integration for ocular following in macaque monkeys. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  K. Kawano,et al.  The effects of prolonged viewing of motion on short-latency ocular following responses , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Leo L. Lui,et al.  Spatial and temporal frequency selectivity of neurons in the middle temporal visual area of new world monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[13]  Aya Takemura,et al.  Deficits in Short-Latency Tracking Eye Movements after Chemical Lesions in Monkey Cortical Areas MT and MST , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  M. Ibbotson,et al.  Enhanced motion sensitivity follows saccadic suppression in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque cortex. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Aya Takemura,et al.  Neuronal responses in MST reflect the post-saccadic enhancement of short-latency ocular following responses , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Initial ocular following in humans: A response to first-order motion energy , 2005, Vision Research.

[17]  Dora E Angelaki,et al.  Similar kinematic properties for ocular following and smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  R. Krauzlis Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Nicholas J. Priebe,et al.  The Neural Representation of Speed in Macaque Area MT/V5 , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  F. Miles,et al.  Short-latency ocular following in humans is dependent on absolute (rather than relative) binocular disparity , 2003, Vision Research.

[21]  A. Takemura,et al.  Sensory-to-motor processing of the ocular-following response , 2002, Neuroscience Research.

[22]  Eric Castet,et al.  Parallel Motion Processing for the Initiation of Short-Latency Ocular Following in Humans , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Aya Takemura,et al.  Visually Driven Eye Movements Elicited at Ultra‐short Latency Are Severely Impaired by MST Lesions , 2002, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[24]  C Busettini,et al.  Short-latency ocular following in humans: sensitivity to binocular disparity , 2001, Vision Research.

[25]  Alexander Thiele,et al.  Speed skills: measuring the visual speed analyzing properties of primate MT neurons , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  E. Castet,et al.  Temporal dynamics of motion integration for the initiation of tracking eye movements at ultra-short latencies , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.

[27]  A. Takemura,et al.  The effect of disparity on the very earliest ocular following responses and the initial neuronal activity in monkey cortical area MST , 2000, Neuroscience Research.

[28]  K. Kawano,et al.  Role of the pretectal nucleus of the optic tract in short-latency ocular following responses in monkeys , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  F A Miles,et al.  The neural processing of 3‐D visual information: evidence from eye movements , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[30]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[31]  S. Yamane,et al.  Neural activity in cortical area MST of alert monkey during ocular following responses. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[32]  F A Miles,et al.  Short latency ocular-following responses in man , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.

[33]  F A Miles,et al.  Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. II. Dependence on a prior saccadic eye movement. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  F. A. Miles,et al.  Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. I. Dependence on temporospatial properties of visual input. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  F A Miles,et al.  Short-latency ocular following responses of monkey. III. Plasticity. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  H. Collewijn Optokinetic eye movements in the rabbit: input-output relations. , 1969, Vision research.