Assembly, tuning, and transfer of action systems in infants and robots

This paper seeks to foster a discussion on whether experiments with machines can inform theory in infant motor development and specifically 1 / how the interactions among the parts of a system, including the nervous and musculoskeletal systems and the forces acting on the body, induce organizational changes in the whole, and 2 / how exploratory behavior and selective informational signals at the time scale of skill learning may allow behavior to become stabilized at the longer time scale of development. The paper describes how three generative principles, inspired from developmental biology and shown to underlie the dynamics of infants learning to bounce in a Jolly Jumper, were broken into a set of mechanisms suitable for controlling a robotic system and resulted in a similar developmental profile. A comparison of infant and robot data leads to a set of criteria for improving the usefulness of robotic studies.

[1]  S. Grillner Neurobiological bases of rhythmic motor acts in vertebrates. , 1985, Science.

[2]  A. Winfree The geometry of biological time , 1991 .

[3]  Luc Berthouze,et al.  Passive compliance for a RC servo-controlled bouncing robot , 2006, Adv. Robotics.

[4]  E. Goldfield Emergent Forms: Origins and Early Development of Human Action and Perception , 1995 .

[5]  R. Raff,et al.  Resynthesizing evolutionary and developmental biology. , 1996, Developmental biology.

[6]  B. Vereijken,et al.  Free(z)ing Degrees of Freedom in Skill Acquisition , 1992 .

[7]  S. Carroll Endless forms most beautiful : the new science of evo devo and the making of the animal kingdom , 2005 .

[8]  A. Needham,et al.  Shared challenges in object perception for robots and infants , 2008 .

[9]  Eugene C. Goldfield,et al.  A Dynamical Systems Approach to Infant Oral Feeding and Dysphagia: From Model System to Therapeutic Medical Device , 2007 .

[10]  Yasuo Kuniyoshi,et al.  Emergence and Categorization of Coordinated Visual Behavior Through Embodied Interaction , 1998, Machine Learning.

[11]  B. Webb,et al.  Can robots make good models of biological behaviour? , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[12]  Barbara Webb,et al.  Robots in invertebrate neuroscience , 2002, Nature.

[13]  C. D. Gelatt,et al.  Optimization by Simulated Annealing , 1983, Science.

[14]  Fiona E. N. LeBeau,et al.  Microcircuits in action – from CPGs to neocortex , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  N. A. Bernshteĭn The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1967 .

[16]  G. Edelman,et al.  Solving Bernstein's problem: a proposal for the development of coordinated movement by selection. , 1993, Child development.

[17]  E. Goldfield Transition from Rocking to Crawling: Postural Constraints on Infant Movement. , 1989 .

[18]  Esther Thelen,et al.  Three-Month-Old Infants Can Learn Task-Specific Patterns of Interlimb Coordination , 1994 .

[19]  S. Schaal,et al.  Rhythmic arm movement is not discrete , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  Luc Berthouze,et al.  On the Interplay Between Morphological, Neural, and Environmental Dynamics: A Robotic Case Study , 2002, Adapt. Behav..

[21]  E. Thelen,et al.  The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perseverative reaching , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[22]  S. L. Hong,et al.  Practice effects on local and global dynamics of the ski-simulator task , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  Leslie Pack Kaelbling,et al.  Ecological Robotics , 1998, Adapt. Behav..

[24]  R. FitzHugh Impulses and Physiological States in Theoretical Models of Nerve Membrane. , 1961, Biophysical journal.

[25]  B. Kay,et al.  Infant bouncing: the assembly and tuning of action systems. , 1993, Child development.

[26]  Luc Berthouze,et al.  Motor Skill Acquisition Under Environmental Perturbations: On the Necessity of Alternate Freezing and Freeing of Degrees of Freedom , 2004, Adapt. Behav..

[27]  E Thelen,et al.  Developmental origins of motor coordination: leg movements in human infants. , 1985, Developmental psychobiology.

[28]  Luc Berthouze,et al.  Robust robot bouncing: passive compliance and flexible phase locking , 2005 .

[29]  N. Metropolis,et al.  Equation of State Calculations by Fast Computing Machines , 1953, Resonance.

[30]  P. Wolff,et al.  A Dynamical Systems Perspective on Infant Action and its Development , 2008 .

[31]  W. H. Warren The dynamics of perception and action. , 2006, Psychological review.

[32]  Robert Platt,et al.  Extending fingertip grasping to whole body grasping , 2003, 2003 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation (Cat. No.03CH37422).

[33]  K. Adolph,et al.  Learning to crawl. , 1998, Child development.

[34]  Giulio Sandini,et al.  Developmental robotics: a survey , 2003, Connect. Sci..

[35]  M. Laubichler Does EvoDevo equal regulatory evolution. Review of: Carroll, Sean B.: Endless forms most beautiful: the new science of Evo Devo and the making of the animal kingdom. New York [u.a.]: Norton 2005 , 2006 .

[36]  A. M. Turing,et al.  The chemical basis of morphogenesis , 1952, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences.

[37]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  Complex systems and human movement , 2006, Complex..

[38]  Stuart A. Kauffman,et al.  The origins of order , 1993 .

[39]  Donald E Ingber,et al.  Mechanical control of tissue morphogenesis during embryological development. , 2006, The International journal of developmental biology.

[40]  Bernard P. Zeigler,et al.  Theory of modeling and simulation , 1976 .

[41]  Luc Berthouze,et al.  Robot Bouncing: On the Synergy Between Neural and Body-Environment Dynamics , 2004, Embodied Artificial Intelligence.

[42]  J. Elman Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small , 1993, Cognition.