Frontal Theta Activity Supports Detecting Mismatched Information in Visual Working Memory

During the comparison stage of visual working memory (VWM) processing, detecting the mismatch between the external sensory input and internal representations is a crucial cognitive ability for human, but the neural mechanism behind it remains largely unclear. The present study investigated the role of frontal theta power in detecting the mismatched information in VWM in a delayed matching task. A control task required to compare two simultaneously presented visual figures was also designed as a contrast to exclude the possibility that frontal theta activity just reflecting the non-memory-related behavioral conflicts. To better characterize the control mechanisms shaped by the frontal theta oscillation in human VWM, colored shapes were adopted as materials while both the task-relevant shape feature and task-irrelevant color feature could be mismatched. We found that the response times of participants were significantly delayed under the relevant- and irrelevant-mismatch conditions in both tasks and the conjunction-mismatch condition in delayed matching task. While our EEG data showed that increased frontal theta power was only observed under the relevant- and conjunction-mismatch conditions in the delayed matching task, but not the control task. These findings suggest that the frontal distributed theta activity observed here reflects the detection of mismatched information during the comparison stage of VWM, rather than the response-related conflicts. Furthermore, it is consistent with the proposal that theta-band oscillation can act as a control mechanism in working memory function so that the target-mismatched information in VWM could be successfully tracked. We also propose a possible processing structure to explain the neural dynamics underlying the mismatch detection process in VWM.

[1]  C. W. Eriksen,et al.  Are nominal same-different matches slower due to differences in level of processing or to response competition? , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Attentional distribution in the visual field duringsame-different judgments as assessed by response competition , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  G. Sperling,et al.  Information transfer in iconic memory experiments. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[5]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  The capacity of visual working memory for features and conjunctions , 1997, Nature.

[6]  S. H. Myhre,et al.  Representation of orientation and spatial frequency in perception and memory: a choice reaction-time analysis. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[8]  M. Chun,et al.  Organization of visual short-term memory. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[9]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Response Conflict : Effects of Response Modality and Processing Domain , 2022 .

[10]  S. Raghavachari,et al.  Gating of Human Theta Oscillations by a Working Memory Task , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain. , 2001, Cerebral Cortex.

[12]  A. Baddeley Is working memory still working? , 2001, The American psychologist.

[13]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  O. Jensen,et al.  Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[15]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Theta coupling in the human electroencephalogram during a working memory task , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[16]  M. Laine,et al.  Effects of normal aging on event-related desynchronization/synchronization during a memory task in humans , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[17]  S. Makeig,et al.  Mining event-related brain dynamics , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[18]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural correlates of change detection and change blindness in a working memory task. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Jaroslaw Zygierewicz,et al.  On the statistical significance of event-related EEG desynchronization and synchronization in the time-frequency plane , 2004, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[20]  M. Eimer,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of change detection. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[21]  Chen Shuo The Capacity of Visual Working Memory for Motion Direction of Objects , 2006 .

[22]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  Visual short term memory , 2007, Scholarpedia.

[23]  C. Patrick,et al.  Externalizing Psychopathology and the Error-Related Negativity , 2007, Psychological science.

[24]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[25]  X. Weng,et al.  A mismatch process in brief delayed matching-to-sample task: an fMRI study , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Ashleigh M. Richard,et al.  Establishing object correspondence across eye movements: Flexible use of spatiotemporal and surface feature information , 2008, Cognition.

[27]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Automatic guidance of attention from working memory , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Jed A. Meltzer,et al.  Effects of Working Memory Load on Oscillatory Power in Human Intracranial EEG , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[29]  Ashleigh M. Richard,et al.  Understanding the function of visual short-term memory: transsaccadic memory, object correspondence, and gaze correction. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[30]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[31]  Simon Hanslmayr,et al.  The Electrophysiological Dynamics of Interference during the Stroop Task , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  A Mouraux,et al.  Across-trial averaging of event-related EEG responses and beyond. , 2008, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[33]  Edward F. Ester,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Stimulus-Specific Delay Activity in Human Primary Visual Cortex , 2022 .

[34]  G. Woodman,et al.  The comparison of visual working memory representations with perceptual inputs. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Control mechanisms in working memory: A possible function of EEG theta oscillations , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[37]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Neural Oscillations Associated with Item and Temporal Order Maintenance in Working Memory , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Theta Dynamics Reveal Domain-specific Control over Stimulus and Response Conflict , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  Fan Wu,et al.  Robust object-based encoding in visual working memory. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[40]  Zhiguo Zhang,et al.  Distinct Features of Auditory Steady-State Responses as Compared to Transient Event-Related Potentials , 2013, PloS one.

[41]  Charan Ranganath,et al.  Frontal midline theta oscillations during working memory maintenance and episodic encoding and retrieval , 2014, NeuroImage.

[42]  B. Postle,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[43]  Edward F. Ester,et al.  Parietal and Frontal Cortex Encode Stimulus-Specific Mnemonic Representations during Visual Working Memory , 2015, Neuron.

[44]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The Representational Basis of Working Memory. , 2016, Current topics in behavioral neurosciences.