Neural Basis of Embodiment: Distinct Contributions of Temporoparietal Junction and Extrastriate Body Area

Embodiment, the sense of being localized within one's physical body, is a fundamental aspect of the self. Recently, researchers have started to show that self and body processing require distinct brain mechanisms, suggesting two posterior brain regions as key loci: the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), which is involved in self processing and multisensory integration of body-related information; and the extrastriate body area (EBA), which responds selectively to human bodies and body parts. Here we used evoked potential mapping and a distributed linear inverse solution to show that activations in EBA and TPJ code differentially for embodiment and self location, because the location and timing of brain activation depended on whether mental imagery is performed with mentally embodied (EBA) or disembodied (TPJ) self location. In a second experiment, we showed that only EBA activation, related to embodied self location, but not TPJ activation, related to disembodied self location, was modified by the subjects' body position during task performance (supine or sitting). This suggests that embodied self location and actual body location share neural mechanisms. Collectively, these data show that distributed brain activity at the EBA and TPJ as well as their timing are crucial for the coding of the self as embodied and as spatially situated within the human body.

[1]  M. Corballis,et al.  What’s up in mental rotation? , 1976 .

[2]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Reference-free identification of components of checkerboard-evoked multichannel potential fields. , 1980, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  L M Parsons,et al.  Imagined spatial transformation of one's body. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[4]  F. Perrin,et al.  Mapping of scalp potentials by surface spline interpolation. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[5]  E. Gonzalez,et al.  Cortical somatosensory evoked potentials: effects of positional changes. , 1989, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[6]  O. Devinsky,et al.  Autoscopic phenomena with seizures. , 1989, Archives of neurology.

[7]  L. Parsons Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  T. Dening,et al.  Autoscopic Phenomena , 1994, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[9]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation , 1995, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[10]  G. Fein,et al.  P50 event-related brain potential amplitude and suppression measurements recorded with subjects seated versus supine , 1997, Biological Psychiatry.

[11]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. , 1998, Science.

[12]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Functional MRI of galvanic vestibular stimulation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  H Okada,et al.  Brain activation during maintenance of standing postures in humans. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  Alain Berthoz,et al.  A fronto-parietal system for computing the egocentric spatial frame of reference in humans , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Imagined transformations of bodies: an fMRI investigation , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Martin J. Farrell,et al.  The automatic updating of egocentric spatial relationships and its impairment due to right posterior cortical lesions , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Activation in Human MT/MST by Static Images with Implied Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  P. Sinha,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  J. Decety,et al.  Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[20]  S. Morand,et al.  Electric source imaging of human brain functions , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[21]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Human Body , 2001 .

[22]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[23]  M. Conway Sensory-perceptual episodic memory and its context: autobiographical memory. , 2001, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[24]  O. Blanke,et al.  Neuropsychology: Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions , 2002, Nature.

[25]  Rüdiger Wenzel,et al.  Human Vestibular Cortex as Identified with Caloric Stimulation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2002, NeuroImage.

[26]  Jean Decety,et al.  Leader or follower? Involvement of the inferior parietal lobule in agency , 2002, Neuroreport.

[27]  J. Haxby,et al.  Parallel Visual Motion Processing Streams for Manipulable Objects and Human Movements , 2002, Neuron.

[28]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion , 2002, Neuron.

[29]  C. Frith,et al.  Experiencing Oneself vs Another Person as Being the Cause of an Action: The Neural Correlates of the Experience of Agency , 2002, NeuroImage.

[30]  M. Jeannerod The mechanism of self-recognition in humans , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[32]  Nikolaus F Troje,et al.  Reference Frames for Orientation Anisotropies in Face Recognition and Biological-Motion Perception , 2003, Perception.

[33]  Stella Christie,et al.  Four types of visual mental imagery processing in upright and tilted observers. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[34]  T. Metzinger Being No One: The Self-Model Theory of Subjectivity , 2004 .

[35]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Shared representations between self and other: a social cognitive neuroscience view , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  G. Fink,et al.  Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  Michael Erb,et al.  The neural correlates of perceiving one's own movements , 2003, NeuroImage.

[38]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Electrical neuroimaging based on biophysical constraints , 2004, NeuroImage.

[39]  C. Michel,et al.  Noninvasive Localization of Electromagnetic Epileptic Activity. II. Demonstration of Sublobar Accuracy in Patients with Simultaneous Surface and Depth Recordings , 2004, Brain Topography.

[40]  C. Michel,et al.  Noninvasive Localization of Electromagnetic Epileptic Activity. I. Method Descriptions and Simulations , 2004, Brain Topography.

[41]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  M. Murray,et al.  EEG source imaging , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[43]  J. Vignal,et al.  Semiologic Value of Ictal Autoscopy , 2004, Epilepsia.

[44]  Brian Levine,et al.  The Functional Neuroanatomy of Episodic and Semantic Autobiographical Remembering: A Prospective Functional MRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  Cosimo Urgesi,et al.  Magnetic Stimulation of Extrastriate Body Area Impairs Visual Processing of Nonfacial Body Parts , 2004, Current Biology.

[46]  O. Blanke,et al.  Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Visual and action cues contribute to the self–other distinction , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  Margot J. Taylor,et al.  N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences between object and face processing using ERPs. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[49]  O. Blanke,et al.  The Out-of-Body Experience: Disturbed Self-Processing at the Temporo-Parietal Junction , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[50]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Grabbing your ear: rapid auditory-somatosensory multisensory interactions in low-level sensory cortices are not constrained by stimulus alignment. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[52]  Olaf Blanke,et al.  Out-of-body experience, heautoscopy, and autoscopic hallucination of neurological origin Implications for neurocognitive mechanisms of corporeal awareness and self-consciousness , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[53]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Is the extrastriate body area involved in motor actions? , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Linking Out-of-Body Experience and Self Processing to Mental Own-Body Imagery at the Temporoparietal Junction , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  Lindsey J. Powell,et al.  My body or yours? The effect of visual perspective on cortical body representations. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  Shinya Yamamoto,et al.  Effects of the gravitational vertical on the visual perception of reversible figures , 2006, Neuroscience Research.