The temporal features of self-referential processing evoked by national flag

The present study explored the neural correlates underlying the psychological processing of self-national flag. Event-related potentials were recorded for national flags while subjects performed a three-stimulus oddball task. The results showed that self-referential stimulus (self-national flag) elicited longer N1 latency and larger N2 amplitude than did non-self-referential stimuli (the familiar and unfamiliar flags). Furthermore, larger P3 amplitudes were showed for self-referential stimulus than for familiar and unfamiliar stimuli. Moreover, in terms of lateralization, the self-referential effect was more obvious on the left region sites. Thus, the present study showed that the self-referential effect not only occurred in late P3 processing stage, but also in early N1 and N2 processing stages, and further demonstrated a left laterality for self-referential processing by using the self-national flag as the self-relevant stimulus.

[1]  John Polich,et al.  P300 and response time from a manual Stroop task , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[2]  T W Picton,et al.  The P300 Wave of the Human Event‐Related Potential , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[3]  Hong Li,et al.  The timing of cognitive control in partially incongruent categorization , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[4]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. , 1998 .

[5]  Jiajin Yuan,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of the collective self-relevant effect , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[6]  Antao Chen,et al.  Gender differences in behavioral inhibitory control: ERP evidence from a two-choice oddball task. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[7]  H Pratt,et al.  P300 in response to the subject's own name. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  B. G. Rule,et al.  Memory for personally relevant information. , 1983 .

[9]  Blair T. Johnson,et al.  The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  Seth J. Gillihan,et al.  Is self special? A critical review of evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[11]  T. Onitsuka,et al.  P300 in response to the subject's own face , 1998, Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[12]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials , 1982, Biological Psychology.

[13]  P. Churchland Self-Representation in Nervous Systems , 2002, Science.

[14]  G. Winocur,et al.  In Search of the Self: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1999 .

[15]  Zhang Li,et al.  Dynamic Cultural Influences on Neural Representations of the Self , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  Stefan R Schweinberger,et al.  What's special about personally familiar faces? A multimodal approach. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[17]  Jiang Qiu,et al.  Spatiotemporal cortical activation underlying self-referencial processing evoked by self-hand , 2010, Biological Psychology.

[18]  Qingguo Ma,et al.  P300 and categorization in brand extension , 2008, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  C. Carter,et al.  The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  David M. Messick,et al.  Codes of Conduct: Behavioral Research into Business Ethics. , 1998 .

[21]  Antonio Damasio,et al.  Feelings of Emotion and the Self , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[22]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Mike or me? Self-recognition in a split-brain patient , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  H. Gray,et al.  P300 as an index of attention to self-relevant stimuli , 2004 .

[24]  F. Bermpohl,et al.  Cortical midline structures and the self , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  M. Brewer The Social Self: On Being the Same and Different at the Same Time , 1991 .

[26]  G. Northoff,et al.  Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: a transcultural neuroimaging approach , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  Georg Northoff,et al.  Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self , 2006, NeuroImage.

[28]  Enoch Callaway,et al.  The effect of attentional effort on visual evoked potential N1 latency , 1982, Psychiatry Research.

[29]  Yue-Jia Luo,et al.  Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: An ERP study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[30]  S. Klein,et al.  The nature of self-referent encoding: The contributions of elaborative and organizational processes. , 1988 .

[31]  Reinhard Dengler,et al.  Differential effects of two motor tasks on ERPs in an auditory classification task: evidence of shared cognitive resources , 1998, Neuroscience Research.

[32]  E. Vogel,et al.  The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[33]  D. Carlson,et al.  Self-reference and group membership: evidence for a group-reference effect , 2002 .

[34]  Hideki Ohira,et al.  An ERP study on self-relevant object recognition , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[35]  V. Johnston,et al.  Multiple P3s to emotional stimuli and their theoretical significance. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[36]  B. Wagar,et al.  Culture, memory, and the self: An analysis of the personal and collective self in long-term memory , 2003 .

[37]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence of Implicit and Explicit Categorization Processes , 2000 .

[38]  W. Schiefenhövel,et al.  The Biological Evolution of Religious Mind and Behavior , 2009 .

[39]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  Shihui Han Religious Belief and Neurocognitive Processes of the Self , 2009 .

[41]  Hong Li,et al.  The Temporal Features of Self-referential Processing Evoked by Chinese Handwriting , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Howard G. Lavine,et al.  Waving the flag: National symbolism, social identity, and political engagement , 2007 .

[43]  G. Northoff,et al.  Understanding the self: a cultural neuroscience approach. , 2009, Progress in brain research.

[44]  K. Kiehl,et al.  Neural sources involved in auditory target detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[45]  Sik Hung Ng,et al.  Dynamic bicultural brains: fMRI study of their flexible neural representation of self and significant others in response to culture primes , 2010 .

[46]  D. Winter,et al.  Sowing Patriotism, But Reaping Nationalism? Consequences of Exposure to the American Flag , 2008 .

[47]  Artur Marchewka,et al.  How multiple repetitions influence the processing of self-, famous and unknown names and faces: an ERP study. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[48]  Hubert D. Zimmer,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of visually processing subject's own name , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[49]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[50]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.