Stereoscopy and the Human Visual System.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Robert S Allison | Simon J Watt | Martin S Banks | Jenny C A Read | M. Banks | S. Watt | J. Read | R. Allison
[1] H H Bülthoff,et al. Integration of depth modules: stereo and shading. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[2] D. Knill. Learning Bayesian priors for depth perception. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[3] Andrew B. Watson,et al. Window of visibility: a psychophysical theory of fidelity in time-sampled visual motion displays , 1986 .
[4] D. Knill. Robust cue integration: a Bayesian model and evidence from cue-conflict studies with stereoscopic and figure cues to slant. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[5] Rent M. Daum,et al. CLINICAL MANAGEMENT OF BINOCULAR VISION: HETEROPHORIC, ACCOMMODATIVE AND EYE MOVEMENT DISORDERS , 1994 .
[6] David C. Burr,et al. How does binocular delay give information about depth? , 1979, Vision Research.
[7] J E Sheedy,et al. Phoria, Vergence, and Fixation Disparity in Oculomotor Problems , 1977, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.
[8] Charless C. Fowlkes,et al. Natural-Scene Statistics Predict How the Figure–Ground Cue of Convexity Affects Human Depth Perception , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[9] Martin S. Banks,et al. Extra-retinal and perspective cues cause the small range of the induced effect , 1998, Vision Research.
[10] M F Bradshaw,et al. Vertical disparities, differential perspective and binocular stereopsis , 1993, Nature.
[11] W. N. Charman,et al. Pupil Diameter and the Depth-of-field of the Human Eye as Measured by Laser Speckle , 1977 .
[12] M. Mon-Williams,et al. Health issues with virtual reality displays: what we do know and what we don't , 1997, COMG.
[13] J. C. Kotulak,et al. Dynamic interactions between accommodation and convergence are velocity sensitive , 1986, Vision Research.
[14] James A. Crowell,et al. Horizontal and vertical disparity, eye position, and stereoscopic slant perception , 1999, Vision Research.
[15] M F Bradshaw,et al. Disparity Minimisation, Cyclovergence, and the Validity of Nonius Lines as a Technique for Measuring Torsional Alignment , 1999, Perception.
[16] Wendy J Adams,et al. Bayesian modeling of cue interaction: bistability in stereoscopic slant perception. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[17] Masaki Emoto,et al. Two factors in visual fatigue caused by stereoscopic HDTV images , 2004 .
[18] D. H. Kelly. Motion and vision. II. Stabilized spatio-temporal threshold surface. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[19] Hiroshi Ando,et al. Effects of head motion and stereo viewing on perceived glossiness. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[20] M. Goulding,et al. Erratum: Lbx1 and Tlx3 are opposing switches in determining GABAergic versus glutamatergic transmitter phenotypes , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[21] Per Capita,et al. About the authors , 1995, Machine Vision and Applications.
[22] Andrew Glennerster,et al. Latitude and longitude vertical disparities. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[23] Rory A. Fisher,et al. Theory of Statistical Estimation , 1925, Mathematical Proceedings of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[24] Kevin J. MacKenzie,et al. Vergence and accommodation to multiple-image-plane stereoscopic displays: 'Real world' responses with practical image-plane separations? , 2011, Electronic Imaging.
[25] T. Meese,et al. Performance data indicate summation for pictorial depth-cues in slanted surfaces. , 2004, Spatial vision.
[26] S K Rushton,et al. Developing visual systems and exposure to virtual reality and stereo displays: some concerns and speculations about the demands on accommodation and vergence. , 1999, Applied ergonomics.
[27] M. Banks,et al. Estimator Reliability and Distance Scaling in Stereoscopic Slant Perception , 1999, Perception.
[28] Kenneth N. Ogle,et al. INDUCED SIZE EFFECT: I. A NEW PHENOMENON IN BINOCULAR SPACE PERCEPTION ASSOCIATED WITH THE RELATIVE SIZES OF THE IMAGES OF THE TWO EYES , 1938 .
[29] James M. Hillis,et al. Slant from texture and disparity cues: optimal cue combination. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[30] I. Howard,et al. Temporal Aspects of Slant and Inclination Perception , 1998, Perception.
[31] H. C. Longuet-Higgins,et al. A computer algorithm for reconstructing a scene from two projections , 1981, Nature.
[32] W. D. Wright. Physiological Optics , 1958, Nature.
[33] Robert S. Allison,et al. The dynamics of vertical vergence , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[34] B. Rogers,et al. The effect of display size on disparity scaling from differential perspective and vergence cues , 1996, Vision Research.
[35] James Gao,et al. High-speed switchable lens enables the development of a volumetric stereoscopic display. , 2009, Optics express.
[36] George Mather,et al. Depth cue integration: stereopsis and image blur , 2000, Vision Research.
[37] M. Ernst,et al. Focus cues affect perceived depth. , 2005, Journal of vision.
[38] B. Julesz. Binocular depth perception of computer-generated patterns , 1960 .
[39] Maurizio Ardito. Studies of the Influence of Display Size and Picture Brightness on the Preferred Viewing Distance for HDTV Programs , 1994 .
[40] David M. Hoffman,et al. Vergence-accommodation conflicts hinder visual performance and cause visual fatigue. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[41] Sheng Liu,et al. An optical see-through head mounted display with addressable focal planes , 2008, 2008 7th IEEE/ACM International Symposium on Mixed and Augmented Reality.
[42] Martin S. Banks,et al. Misperceptions in stereoscopic displays: a vision science perspective , 2008, APGV '08.
[43] Hans-Peter Seidel,et al. Real-time lens blur effects and focus control , 2010, SIGGRAPH 2010.
[44] I. P. Howard,et al. Effects of stimulus size and eccentricity on horizontal and vertical vergence , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[45] James F. O'Brien,et al. Using blur to affect perceived distance and size , 2010, TOGS.
[46] James J. Clark,et al. Data Fusion for Sensory Information Processing Systems , 1990 .
[47] J. Porrill,et al. Stereopsis, vertical disparity and relief transformations , 1995, Vision Research.
[48] B. Anderson. Depth perception , 2008 .
[49] Martin S. Banks,et al. A stereo display prototype with multiple focal distances , 2004, ACM Trans. Graph..
[50] Robert S. Allison,et al. Analysis of the influence of vertical disparities arising in Toed-in stereoscopic cameras , 2007 .
[51] Induced size effect. I : A new phenomenon in binocular vision associated with the relative sizes of the images in the two eyes , 1938 .
[52] Mtm Marc Lambooij,et al. Visual Discomfort and Visual Fatigue of Stereoscopic Displays: A Review , 2009 .
[53] Martin S Banks,et al. Temporal presentation protocols in stereoscopic displays: Flicker visibility, perceived motion, and perceived depth , 2011, Journal of the Society for Information Display.
[54] R. S Allison,et al. Depth selectivity of vertical fusional mechanisms , 2000, Vision Research.
[55] Inna Tsirlin,et al. Stereoscopic transparency: constraints on the perception of multiple surfaces. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[56] Peter A. Howarth,et al. Visual fatigue caused by viewing stereoscopic motion images: Background, theories, and observations , 2008, Displays.
[57] James M. Hillis,et al. Combining Sensory Information: Mandatory Fusion Within, but Not Between, Senses , 2002, Science.
[58] F. Okano,et al. Repeated vergence adaptation causes the decline of visual functions in watching stereoscopic television , 2005, Journal of Display Technology.
[59] Bernard Mendiburu,et al. 3D Movie Making: Stereoscopic Digital Cinema from Script to Screen , 2009 .
[60] Kevin J. MacKenzie,et al. Vergence and accommodation to multiple-image-plane stereoscopic displays: "real world" responses with practical image-plane separations? , 2012, J. Electronic Imaging.
[61] R. S Allison,et al. Temporal dependencies in resolving monocular and binocular cue conflict in slant perception , 2000, Vision Research.
[62] Mel W. Siegel,et al. Just enough reality: comfortable 3-D viewing via microstereopsis , 2000, IEEE Trans. Circuits Syst. Video Technol..
[63] Ahna R Girshick,et al. Probabilistic combination of slant information: weighted averaging and robustness as optimal percepts. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[64] Tom Geller,et al. Overcoming the Uncanny Valley , 2008, IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications.
[65] M. Morgan,et al. Perception of continuity in stroboscopic motion: A temporal frequency analysis , 1979, Vision Research.
[66] C. WILLIAM TYLER,et al. Depth perception in disparity gratings , 1974, Nature.
[67] Xiao-Fan Feng. LCD motion-blur analysis, perception, and reduction using synchronized backlight flashing , 2006, Electronic Imaging.
[68] E. Brenner,et al. Testing a counter-intuitive prediction of optimal cue combination , 2009, Vision Research.
[69] M. Landy,et al. Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.
[70] Ahna R Girshick,et al. Perception of 3‐D Layout in Stereo Displays , 2009, Information display.
[71] Kevin J. MacKenzie,et al. Accommodation to multiple-focal-plane displays: Implications for improving stereoscopic displays and for accommodation control. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[72] Campbell Fw. A method for measuring the depth of field of the human eye. , 1954 .
[73] Andrew J. Woods,et al. Image distortions in stereoscopic video systems , 1993, Electronic Imaging.
[74] Leo Jan Velthoven,et al. Motion blur reduction for liquid crystal displays: motion-compensated inverse filtering , 2004, IS&T/SPIE Electronic Imaging.
[75] David M. Hoffman,et al. The zone of comfort: Predicting visual discomfort with stereo displays. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[76] A. Fitzgibbon,et al. Humans Ignore Motion and Stereo Cues in Favor of a Fictional Stable World , 2006, Current Biology.