Fatiguing effect of multiple take-offs and landings in regional airline operations.

Fatigue is a risk factor for flight performance and safety in commercial aviation. In US commercial aviation, to help to curb fatigue, the maximum duration of flight duty periods is regulated based on the scheduled start time and the number of flight segments to be flown. There is scientific support for regulating maximum duty duration based on scheduled start time; fatigue is well established to be modulated by circadian rhythms. However, it has not been established scientifically whether the number of flight segments, per se, affects fatigue. To address this science gap, we conducted a randomized, counterbalanced, cross-over study with 24 active-duty regional airline pilots. Objective and subjective fatigue was compared between a 9-hour duty day with multiple take-offs and landings versus a duty day of equal duration with a single take-off and landing. To standardize experimental conditions and isolate the fatiguing effect of the number of segments flown, the entire duty schedules were carried out in a high-fidelity, moving-base, full-flight, regional jet flight simulator. Steps were taken to maintain operational realism, including simulated airplane inspections and acceptance checks, use of realistic dispatch releases and airport charts, real-world air traffic control interactions, etc. During each of the two duty days, 10 fatigue test bouts were administered, which included a 10-minute Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT) assessment of objective fatigue and Samn-Perelli (SP) and Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) assessments of subjective sleepiness/fatigue. Results showed a greater build-up of objective and subjective fatigue in the multi-segment duty day than in the single-segment duty day. With duty start time and duration and other variables that could impact fatigue levels held constant, the greater build-up of fatigue in the multi-segment duty day was attributable specifically to the difference in the number of flight segments flown. Compared to findings in previously published laboratory studies of simulated night shifts and nighttime sleep deprivation, the magnitude of the fatiguing effect of the multiple take-offs and landings was modest. Ratings of flight performance were not significantly reduced for the simulated multi-segment duty day. The US duty and flight time regulations for commercial aviation shorten the maximum duty duration in multi-segment operations by up to 25% depending on the duty start time. The present results represent an important first step in understanding fatigue in multi-segment operations, and provide support for the number of flight segments as a relevant factor in regulating maximum duty duration. Nonetheless, based on our fatigue results, a more moderate reduction in maximum duty duration as a function of the number of flight segments might be considered. However, further research is needed to include investigation of flight safety, and to extend our findings to nighttime operations.

[1]  M. Chee,et al.  Neuroimaging of Sleep and Sleep Disorders: Functional imaging of inter-individual differences in response to sleep deprivation , 2013 .

[2]  T H Monk,et al.  Circadian rhythms in human performance and mood under constant conditions , 1997, Journal of sleep research.

[3]  M. Basner,et al.  Significance of time awake for predicting pilots' fatigue on short‐haul flights: implications for flight duty time regulations , 2014, Journal of sleep research.

[4]  Charli Sargent,et al.  Duty periods with early start times restrict the amount of sleep obtained by short-haul airline pilots. , 2012, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[5]  A. Muzet,et al.  Sleep inertia. , 2000, Sleep medicine reviews.

[6]  Wendy Macdonald,et al.  Effects of workload level and 8- versus 12-h workday duration on test battery performance , 2000 .

[7]  Gregory Belenky,et al.  The efficacy of a restart break for recycling with optimal performance depends critically on circadian timing. , 2011, Sleep.

[8]  D. Dinges,et al.  Psychomotor Vigilance Performance: Neurocognitive Assay Sensitive to Sleep Loss , 2004 .

[9]  D. Dinges,et al.  Sleep Deprivation and Vigilant Attention , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[10]  Maria L. Thomas,et al.  Comparative utility of instruments for monitoring sleepiness‐related performance decrements in the operational environment , 2004, Journal of sleep research.

[11]  D. Dinges,et al.  Psychomotor Vigilance Performance: Neurocognitive Assay Sensitive to Sleep Loss , 2004 .

[12]  B. M. Hartzler,et al.  Battling Fatigue in Aviation: Recent Advancements in Research and Practice , 2012 .

[13]  D. Dijk,et al.  Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance , 1992, Journal of sleep research.

[14]  H. V. Van Dongen,et al.  Individual differences in cognitive vulnerability to fatigue in the laboratory and in the workplace. , 2011, Progress in brain research.

[15]  D. Dinges,et al.  Dynamic circadian modulation in a biomathematical model for the effects of sleep and sleep loss on waking neurobehavioral performance. , 2013, Sleep.

[16]  D. Dinges,et al.  Systematic interindividual differences in neurobehavioral impairment from sleep loss: evidence of trait-like differential vulnerability. , 2004, Sleep.

[17]  D. Dinges,et al.  Cognitive workload and sleep restriction interact to influence sleep homeostatic responses. , 2014, Sleep.

[18]  P. Whitney,et al.  Feedback Blunting: Total Sleep Deprivation Impairs Decision Making that Requires Updating Based on Feedback. , 2015, Sleep.

[19]  G. Belenky,et al.  Effects of sleep/wake history and circadian phase on proposed pilot fatigue safety performance indicators , 2015, Journal of sleep research.

[20]  Sherwood W Samn,et al.  Estimating Aircrew Fatigue: A Technique with Application to Airlift Operations , 1982 .

[21]  S. Doran,et al.  Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. , 2001, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[22]  D. Dinges,et al.  A new mathematical model for the homeostatic effects of sleep loss on neurobehavioral performance. , 2009, Journal of theoretical biology.

[23]  T. Åkerstedt,et al.  Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. , 1990, The International journal of neuroscience.

[24]  E. Broadbent,et al.  Pilot fatigue in short-haul operations: effects of number of sectors, duty length, and time of day. , 2007, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[25]  H. V. Van Dongen,et al.  Occupational fatigue, underlying sleep and circadian mechanisms, and approaches to fatigue risk management , 2013 .

[26]  Mick B Spencer,et al.  Fatigue in two-pilot operations: implications for flight and duty time limitations. , 2008, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[27]  Alain Muzet,et al.  Sleep inertia. , 2000, Sleep medicine reviews.

[28]  Gregory Belenky,et al.  Pilot fatigue: relationships with departure and arrival times, flight duration, and direction. , 2014, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[29]  S. Hursh,et al.  Chapter 67 – Fatigue, Performance, Errors, and Accidents , 2010 .

[30]  Jürgen Wenzel,et al.  Pilot workload during approaches: comparison of simulated standard and noise-abatement profiles. , 2009, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[31]  G. Belenky,et al.  Investigating the temporal dynamics and underlying mechanisms of cognitive fatigue. , 2011 .

[32]  E. F. Colecchia,et al.  Individual differences in subjective and objective alertness during sleep deprivation are stable and unrelated. , 2003, American journal of physiology. Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology.

[33]  Hans P. A. Van Dongen,et al.  Integrated fatigue modeling in crew rostering and operations , 2013 .

[34]  Hans P. A. Van Dongen,et al.  Morning-type and evening-type individuals differ in the phase position of their endogenous circadian oscillator , 1996, Neuroscience Letters.

[35]  D. Dinges,et al.  The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. , 2003, Sleep.

[36]  J. Caldwell,et al.  Efficacy of Dexedrine for maintaining aviator performance during 64 hours of sustained wakefulness: a simulator study. , 2000, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[37]  Derk-Jan Dijk,et al.  Paradoxical timing of the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity serves to consolidate sleep and wakefulness in humans , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[38]  Andreas Haslbeck,et al.  Effects of Workload on Measures of Sustained Attention During a Flight Simulator Night Mission , 2015 .

[39]  Greg Maislin,et al.  Dealing with inter-individual differences in the temporal dynamics of fatigue and performance: importance and techniques. , 2004, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[40]  Samira Bourgeois-Bougrine,et al.  Sleep and sleepiness during an ultra long-range flight operation between the Middle East and United States. , 2012, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[41]  Drew Dawson,et al.  Managing fatigue: it's about sleep. , 2005, Sleep medicine reviews.

[42]  S. Daan,et al.  Timing of human sleep: recovery process gated by a circadian pacemaker. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[43]  J. Caldwell,et al.  Modafinil's effects on simulator performance and mood in pilots during 37 h without sleep. , 2004, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[44]  John A. Detre,et al.  Imaging brain fatigue from sustained mental workload: An ASL perfusion study of the time-on-task effect , 2010, NeuroImage.