The Influence of Memory Load Upon Delay-Interval Activity in a Working-Memory Task: An Event-Related Functional MRI Study

We conducted two fMRI studies to investigate the sensitivity of delay-period activity to changes in memory load during a delayed-recognition task for faces. In Experiment 1, each trial began with the presentation of a memory array consisting of one, two, or three faces that lasted for 3 sec. A 15-sec delay period followed during which no stimuli were present. The delay interval concluded with a one-face probe to which subjects made a button press response indicating whether this face was part of the memory array. Experiment 2 was similar in design except that the delay period was lengthened to 24 sec, and the memory array consisted of only one or three faces. We hypothesized that memory maintenance processes that spanned the delay interval would be revealed by their sensitivity to memory load. Long delay intervals were employed to temporally dissociate phasic activity engendered by the memory array from sustained activity reflecting maintenance. Regions of interest (ROIs) were defined anatomically for the superior frontal gyri (SFG), middle frontal gyri (MFG), and inferior frontal gyri (IFG), intraparietal sulci (IPS), and fusiform gyri (FFG) on a subject-by-subject basis. The mean time course of activity was determined for all voxels within these regions and for that subset of voxels within each ROI that correlated significantly with an empirically determined reference waveform. In both experiments, memory load significantly influenced activation 6-9 sec following the onset of the memory array with larger amplitude responses for higher load levels. Responses were greatest within MFG, IPS, and FFG. In both experiments, however, these load-sensitive differences declined over successive time intervals and were no longer significant at the end of the delay interval. Although insensitive to our load manipulation, sustained activation was present at the conclusion of the delay interval within MFG and other prefrontal regions. IPS delay activity returned to prestimulus baseline levels prior to the end of the delay period in Experiment 2, but not in Experiment 1. Within FFG, delay activity returned to prestimulus baseline levels prior to the conclusion of the delay interval in both experiments. Thus, while phasic processes engendered by the memory array were strongly affected by memory load, no evidence for load-sensitive delay-spanning maintenance processes was obtained.

[1]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  R. Desimone,et al.  A neural mechanism for working and recognition memory in inferior temporal cortex. , 1991, Science.

[3]  R. Desimone,et al.  Activity of neurons in anterior inferior temporal cortex during a short- term memory task , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[5]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of human prefrontal cortex activation during a spatial working memory task. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Activation of the prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[7]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Neural correlates of working memory in a visual letter monitoring task: an fMRI study , 1995, Neuroreport.

[8]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[9]  Cheryl L. Grady,et al.  Hemispheric differences in neural systems for face working memory: A PET‐rCBF study , 1995 .

[10]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  M. A. Steinmetz,et al.  Neuronal activity in posterior parietal area 7a during the delay periods of a spatial memory task. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[14]  A Baddeley,et al.  The fractionation of working memory. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.

[16]  D. Manoach,et al.  Prefrontal cortex fMRI signal changes are correlated with working memory load , 1997, Neuroreport.

[17]  J. Desmond,et al.  Lobular Patterns of Cerebellar Activation in Verbal Working-Memory and Finger-Tapping Tasks as Revealed by Functional MRI , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory , 1997, Nature.

[20]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Verbal Working Memory Load Affects Regional Brain Activation as Measured by PET , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[21]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[22]  R Turner,et al.  Blood oxygenation level dependent signal time courses during prolonged visual stimulation. , 1998, Magnetic resonance imaging.

[23]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Human Brain Mapping 6:14–32(1998) � Dissociation of Mnemonic and Perceptual Processes During Spatial and Nonspatial Working Memory Using fMRI , 2022 .

[24]  G. Sammer,et al.  Working memory load and EEG-dynamics as revealed by point correlation dimension analysis. , 1999, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[25]  B. Postle,et al.  Functional neuroanatomical double dissociation of mnemonic and executive control processes contributing to working memory performance. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  M D'Esposito,et al.  The roles of prefrontal brain regions in components of working memory: effects of memory load and individual differences. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[27]  R. Coppola,et al.  Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Rehearsal in Spatial Working Memory: Evidence From Neuroimaging , 1999 .

[29]  B. Postle,et al.  Maintenance versus Manipulation of Information Held in Working Memory: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[30]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI , 2000, NeuroImage.

[31]  R. Passingham,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? , 2000, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..

[32]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Inactivation of parietal and prefrontal cortex reveals interdependence of neural activity during memory-guided saccades. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.