Making people feel guilty in conversations: Techniques and correlates.

Communication plays an important role in eliciting and shaping people's emotions. Yet surprisingly little empirical or conceptual work has explored how social interaction encourages or discourages various emotions. This article summarizes a series of four studies which examined how people, in conversations, go about generating one emotion—that of guilt—in others. In the first study, a typology of 12 guilt moves was inductively devised. The most common ways of making another feel guilty were: reminding the other of his or her obligation to a relationship, listing sacrifices one has made for the other, reminding the other of his or her role responsibilities, comparing the other to someone else, and questioning the other. The primary reason for using guilt in conversations was persuasion. In the second study, the typology's generalizability was confirmed. Findings also revealed that the likelihood of using guilt in conversations increases with greater relationship intimacy and that people believe others are more likely to use guilt than themselves. The 12 techniques for guilt elicitation were examined in the third study for their independence and dimensionality via multidimensional scaling. The results suggest three underlying dimensions (self-nonself, acquiescence-restrictiveness, and commission-omission) and six potential clusters. The final study conceptualized guilt elicitation and sensitivity as individual differences. A brief, multifactor scale of individuals’ tendency to use guilt, to use it with ease, and to be susceptible to guilt moves was developed. The three dimensions correlated significantly with a number of communication-focused personality variables.

[1]  D. T. Regan,et al.  Voluntary expiation of guilt: a field experiment. , 1972, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  Susan B. Shimanoff Types of emotional disclosures and request compliance between spouses , 1987 .

[3]  D. Buss,et al.  Conflict between the sexes: strategic interference and the evocation of anger and upset. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  B. Weiner An attributional theory of motivation and emotion , 1986 .

[5]  D. Marlowe,et al.  The Approval Motive: Studies in Evaluative Dependence , 1980 .

[6]  Manuel J. Smith,et al.  When I Say No, I Feel Guilty , 1975 .

[7]  K. Scherer,et al.  Experiencing emotion : a cross-cultural study , 1986 .

[8]  Joseph de Rivera,et al.  Emotions as social relationships , 1986 .

[9]  H. Guetzkow Unitizing and categorizing problems in coding qualitative data , 1950 .

[10]  J. Freedman,et al.  Compliance without pressure: The effect of guilt. , 1967 .

[11]  F. Christopher,et al.  Strategies of Influence in Sex and Dating , 1990 .

[12]  Susan B. Shimanoff Rules governing the verbal expression of emotions between married couples , 1985 .

[13]  Eric Berne,et al.  Games People Play , 1964 .

[14]  F. W. Wicker,et al.  Participant descriptions of guilt and shame , 1983 .

[15]  John A. Daly,et al.  The affinity-seeking function of communication , 1984 .

[16]  R. Christie,et al.  Studies in Machiavellianism , 1970 .

[17]  B. Weiner,et al.  Pity, Anger, and Guilt , 1982 .

[18]  J. Averill A CONSTRUCTIVIST VIEW OF EMOTION , 1980 .

[19]  A. Buss,et al.  An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. , 1957, Journal of consulting psychology.

[20]  M. Covington,et al.  The Trouble With Pitfalls: A Reply to Weiner's Critique of Attribution Research. , 1984 .

[21]  A. Gross,et al.  Some effects of guilt on compliance. , 1969, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[22]  M. Rosenberg Society and the adolescent self-image , 1966 .

[23]  Penelope Brown,et al.  Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage , 1989 .

[24]  M. Bulmer Concepts in the Analysis of Qualitative Data , 1979 .

[25]  P. Shaver,et al.  Emotion knowledge: further exploration of a prototype approach. , 1987 .

[26]  M. Lorr,et al.  Four Dimensions Of Assertiveness , 1980 .

[27]  Richard S. Lazarus,et al.  EMOTIONS: A COGNITIVE–PHENOMENOLOGICAL ANALYSIS , 1980 .

[28]  Mark H. Davis Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. , 1983 .

[29]  G. Marwell,et al.  Dimensions of Compliance-Gaining Behavior: An Empirical Analysis , 1967 .

[30]  G. Leech Principles of pragmatics , 1983 .

[31]  M. Knapp,et al.  Perceptions of communication behavior associated with relationship terms , 1980 .

[32]  G. Mandler Mind and Emotion , 1975 .

[33]  A. Buss,et al.  Shyness and sociability. , 1981 .

[34]  D. Buss,et al.  Structure of act-report data: is the five-factor model of personality recaptured? , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[35]  J. Rivera A structural theory of the emotions , 1977 .

[36]  C. Hazan,et al.  Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  M. Covington,et al.  An empirical examination of Weiner's critique of attribution research. , 1984 .

[38]  Gabriele Taylor,et al.  Pride, shame, and guilt , 1985 .

[39]  J. W. Regan,et al.  Guilt, perceived injustice, and altruistic behavior. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  W. Zung A SELF-RATING DEPRESSION SCALE. , 1965, Archives of general psychiatry.

[41]  E. Rawlings Witnessing harm to other: a reassessment of the role of guilt in altruistic behavior. , 1968, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[42]  Renée Edwards,et al.  Evaluators' Perceptions of Teachers' Use of Behavior Alteration Techniques. , 1988 .