Visual imagery without visual experience: evidence from congenitally totally blind people

We explored the ability of congenitally totally blind people (who were contrasted with age-, sex- and education matched blindfolded sighted subjects) to perform tasks which are mediated by visual mental imagery in sighted people. In the first (pictorial) task, subjects had to mentally compare the shape of the outline of three named objects and to indicate the odd-one-out. In the second (spatial) task the participants were asked to memorise the position of a target cube in two- and three-dimensional matrices, based on a sequence of spatially based imagery operations. In addition, during half of the trials of both imagery tasks subjects were required to perform a concurrent finger tapping task, to investigate whether the blind subjects would be more dependent on spatial processing. Although blind participants made significantly more errors than sighted participants, they were well able to perform the spatial imagery task as well as the pictorial imagery task. Interference from the concurrent tapping task affected both groups to the same extent. Our results shed new light on the question whether early visual experience is necessary for performance on visual imagery tasks, and strongly suggest that vision and haptics may share common representations.

[1]  T. Vecchi,et al.  Visuo-spatial imagery in congenitally totally blind people. , 1998, Memory.

[2]  C. Cornoldi,et al.  Individual differences in the capacity limitations of visuospatial short-term memory: Research on sighted and totally congenitally blind people , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[3]  E. M. Craig Role of mental imagery in free recall of deaf, blind, and normal sunjects. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  M. Hallett,et al.  Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects , 1996, Nature.

[5]  Rossana de Beni,et al.  Imagery limitations in totally congenitally blind subjects. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  R. Goebel,et al.  Matching two imagined clocks: the functional anatomy of spatial analysis in the absence of visual stimulation. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[7]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Involvement of visual cortex in tactile discrimination of orientation , 1999, Nature.

[8]  G. S. Marmor,et al.  Mental rotation by the blind: does mental rotation depend on visual imagery? , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[10]  D. Levine,et al.  Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.

[11]  R D Easton,et al.  Do vision and haptics share common representations? Implicit and explicit memory within and between modalities. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  J. Keenan,et al.  Imagery in the congenitally blind: how visual are visual images? , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  N H Kerr,et al.  Locational representation in imagery: The third dimension , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[14]  M. Greenlee,et al.  Cortical activation evoked by visual mental imagery as measured by fMRI , 2000, Neuroreport.

[15]  C. Cornoldi,et al.  Processing Capacity Limitations in Pictorial and Spatial Representations in the Totally Congenitally Blind , 1993, Cortex.

[16]  F. Newcombe,et al.  Selective loss of imagery in a case of visual agnosia , 1992, Neuropsychologia.