Revisiting the effect of quality of graphics on distance judgments in virtual environments: A comparison of verbal reports and blind walking

In immersive virtual environments, judgments of perceived egocentric distance are significantly underestimated, as compared with accurate performance in the real world. Two experiments assessed the influence of graphics quality on two distinct estimates of distance, a visually directed walking task and verbal reports. Experiment 1 demonstrated a similar underestimation of distances walked to previously viewed targets in both low- and high-quality virtual classrooms. In Experiment 2, participants’ verbal judgments underestimated target distances in both graphics quality environments but were more accurate in the high-quality environment, consistent with the subjective impression that high-quality environments seem larger. Contrary to previous results, we suggest that quality of graphics does influence judgments of distance, but only for verbal reports. This behavioral dissociation has implications beyond the context of virtual environments and may reflect a differential use of cues and context for verbal reports and visually directed walking.

[1]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Does the Quality of the Computer Graphics Matter when Judging Distances in Visually Immersive Environments? , 2004, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[2]  William B. Thompson,et al.  HMD calibration and its effects on distance judgments , 2008, APGV '08.

[3]  D. Carey,et al.  Do action systems resist visual illusions? , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  David Waller,et al.  Interaction With an Immersive Virtual Environment Corrects Users' Distance Estimates , 2007, Hum. Factors.

[5]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Measuring Spatial Perception with Spatial Updating and Action , 2008 .

[6]  S. Black,et al.  Intact spatial updating during locomotion after right posterior parietal lesions , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Path Integration Deficits during Linear Locomotion after Human Medial Temporal Lobectomy , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Effects of Stereo Viewing Conditions on Distance Perception in Virtual Environments , 2008, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.

[9]  Christian Büchel,et al.  Spatial updating: how the brain keeps track of changing object locations during observer motion , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[10]  S S Fukusima,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  The influence of feedback on egocentric distance judgments in real and virtual environments , 2006, APGV '06.

[12]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon , 2001, Nature.

[13]  Adam R. Richardson,et al.  The effect of feedback training on distance estimation in virtual environments , 2005 .

[14]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Perceiving distance accurately by a directional process of integrating ground information , 2004, Nature.

[15]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Perceived egocentric distances in real, image-based, and traditional virtual environments , 2002, Proceedings IEEE Virtual Reality 2002.

[17]  J. Lappin,et al.  Environmental context influences visually perceived distance , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Absolute Distance Perception to Locations off the Ground Plane , 2007, Perception.

[19]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  The Influence of Restricted Viewing Conditions on Egocentric Distance Perception: Implications for Real and Virtual Indoor Environments , 2005, Perception.

[20]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Does Energy Expenditure Affect the Perception of Egocentric Distance? A Failure to Replicate Experiment 1 of Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, and Epstein (2003) , 2006, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.

[21]  J. Rieser,et al.  Visual Perception and the Guidance of Locomotion without Vision to Previously Seen Targets , 1990, Perception.

[22]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Judgments of Exocentric Direction in Large-Scale Space , 2004 .

[23]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[24]  Frank H. Durgin,et al.  More recalibration of the perception of linear self-motion , 2010 .

[25]  J. Andre,et al.  Using verbal and blind-walking distance estimates to investigate the two visual systems hypothesis , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[27]  J M Loomis,et al.  Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  A. S. Gilinsky Perceived size and distance in visual space. , 1951, Psychological review.

[29]  James E. Cutting,et al.  Chapter 3 – Perceiving Layout and Knowing Distances: The Integration, Relative Potency, and Contextual Use of Different Information about Depth* , 1995 .

[30]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  Throwing versus walking as indicators of distance perception in similar real and virtual environments , 2005, TAP.

[31]  Jack M. Loomis,et al.  Visual perception of egocentric distance in real and virtual environments. , 2003 .

[32]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visual space perception and visually directed action. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  V. Franz,et al.  Action does not resist visual illusions , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Perceptual Space in the Dark Affected by the Intrinsic Bias of the Visual System , 2006, Perception.

[36]  David Waller,et al.  Correcting distance estimates by interacting with immersive virtual environments: effects of task and available sensory information. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[37]  Peter Willemsen,et al.  The effects of head-mounted display mechanical properties and field of view on distance judgments in virtual environments , 2009, TAP.

[38]  Betty J. Mohler,et al.  Calibration of locomotion resulting from visual motion in a treadmill-based virtual environment , 2007, TAP.

[39]  J. Philbeck,et al.  Comparison of two indicators of perceived egocentric distance under full-cue and reduced-cue conditions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Terrain influences the accurate judgement of distance , 1998, Nature.

[41]  Frank H. Durgin,et al.  Distance Perception and the Visual Horizon in Head-Mounted Displays , 2005, TAP.

[42]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Seeing beyond the Target: Environmental Context Affects Distance Perception , 2007, Perception.

[43]  Wallace J. Sadowski,et al.  Nonvisually Guided Locomotion to a Previously Viewed Target in Real and Virtual Environments , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[44]  Zijiang J. He,et al.  Judging Egocentric Distance on the Ground: Occlusion and Surface Integration , 2004, Perception.