Carrying the past to the future: Distinct brain networks underlie individual differences in human spatial working memory capacity

ABSTRACT Spatial working memory (SWM) relies on the interplay of anatomically separated and interconnected large‐scale brain networks. EEG studies often observe load‐associated sustained negative activity during SWM retention. Yet, whether and how such sustained negative activity in retention relates to network‐specific functional activation/deactivation and relates to individual differences in SWM capacity remain to be elucidated. To cover these gaps, we recorded concurrent EEG‐fMRI data in 70 healthy young adults during the Sternberg delayed‐match‐to‐sample SWM task with three memory load levels. To a subset of participants (N=28) that performed the task properly and had artefact‐free fMRI and EEG data, we employed a novel temporo‐spatial principal component analysis to derive load‐dependent negative slow wave (NSW) from retention‐related event‐related potentials. The associations between NSW responses with SWM capacity were divergent in the higher (N=14) and lower (N=14) SWM capacity groups. Specifically, larger load‐related increase in NSW amplitude was associated with greater SWM capacity for the higher capacity group but lower SWM capacity for the lower capacity group. Furthermore, for the higher capacity group, larger NSW amplitude was related to greater activation in bilateral parietal areas of the fronto‐parietal network (FPN) and greater deactivation in medial frontal gyrus and posterior mid‐cingulate cortex of the default mode network (DMN) during retention. In contrast, the lower capacity group did not show similar pattern. Instead, greater NSW was linked to higher deactivation in right posterior middle temporal gyrus. Our findings shed light on the possible differential EEG‐informed neural network mechanism during memory maintenance underlying individual differences in SWM capacity. HIGHLIGHTSNegative slow wave (NSW) is related to spatial working memory (SWM) load during retentionNSW in the high and low capacity groups showed divergent associations with SWM capacity and network activitiesNSW was related to higher FPN activation and DMN deactivation in the high capacity groupNSW was related to greater temporal deactivation in the low capacity groupDistinct NSW‐related brain networks are related to individual differences in SWM capacities

[1]  Naoyuki Sato,et al.  Cortical networks dynamically emerge with the interplay of slow and fast oscillations for memory of a natural scene , 2015, NeuroImage.

[2]  M. Cannon,et al.  Reduced P300 amplitude during retrieval on a spatial working memory task in a community sample of adolescents who report psychotic symptoms , 2013, BMC Psychiatry.

[3]  Valer Jurcak,et al.  10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: Their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems , 2007, NeuroImage.

[4]  C. Constantinidis,et al.  The neuroscience of working memory capacity and training , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  Chen Bian,et al.  The different oscillation patterns of alpha band in the early and later stages of working memory maintenance , 2016, Neuroscience Letters.

[6]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Exploring the role of the posterior middle temporal gyrus in semantic cognition: Integration of anterior temporal lobe with executive processes , 2016, NeuroImage.

[7]  C. Tenke,et al.  Optimizing PCA methodology for ERP component identification and measurement: theoretical rationale and empirical evaluation , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[8]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordinate-based meta-analysis , 2012, NeuroImage.

[9]  P. Uhlhaas,et al.  Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha–gamma versus theta–gamma codes for distinct WM information? , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  J. Connolly,et al.  Correlating digit span performance and event-related potentials to assess working memory. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[11]  Kartik K. Sreenivasan,et al.  Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memory , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Heinz Holling,et al.  Is functional integration of resting state brain networks an unspecific biomarker for working memory performance? , 2015, NeuroImage.

[13]  Torkel Klingberg,et al.  The role of fronto-parietal and fronto-striatal networks in the development of working memory: a longitudinal study. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[14]  T. Koenig,et al.  Developmental Changes of BOLD Signal Correlations with Global Human EEG Power and Synchronization during Working Memory , 2012, PloS one.

[15]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.

[16]  A. Nobre,et al.  Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  Anna S. Huang,et al.  Load-related brain activation predicts spatial working memory performance in youth aged 9–12 and is associated with executive function at earlier ages , 2015, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  Juan Zhou,et al.  Better Not to Know? Emotion Regulation Fails to Benefit from Affective Cueing , 2016, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[19]  Constantinos I. Siettos,et al.  Granger causality analysis reveals distinct spatio-temporal connectivity patterns in motor and perceptual visuo-spatial working memory , 2014, Front. Comput. Neurosci..

[20]  Joseph Dien,et al.  Evaluating two-step PCA of ERP data with Geomin, Infomax, Oblimin, Promax, and Varimax rotations. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[21]  A. Karim,et al.  Brain Oscillatory Substrates of Visual Short-Term Memory Capacity , 2009, Current Biology.

[22]  R. Ptak The Frontoparietal Attention Network of the Human Brain , 2012, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[23]  Wolf Singer,et al.  Gamma-Band Activity in Human Prefrontal Cortex Codes for the Number of Relevant Items Maintained in Working Memory , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  W. Ma,et al.  Changing concepts of working memory , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Irene E. Nagel,et al.  Microstructure of frontoparietal connections predicts cortical responsivity and working memory performance. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[26]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Cortical capacity constraints for visual working memory: dissociation of fMRI load effects in a fronto-parietal network , 2003, NeuroImage.

[27]  Dieter Vaitl,et al.  Relationship between regional hemodynamic activity and simultaneously recorded EEG‐theta associated with mental arithmetic‐induced workload , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[28]  Robin D. Thomas,et al.  Principal component analysis of the memory load effect in a change detection task , 2015, Vision Research.

[29]  Manfred Herrmann,et al.  Differences between target and non-target probe processing — Combined evidence from fMRI, EEG and fMRI-constrained source analysis , 2015, NeuroImage.

[30]  Joseph Dien,et al.  Applying Principal Components Analysis to Event-Related Potentials: A Tutorial , 2012, Developmental neuropsychology.

[31]  Sohee Park,et al.  Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. , 1992, Archives of general psychiatry.

[32]  A. Stevens,et al.  Functional Brain Network Modularity Captures Inter- and Intra-Individual Variation in Working Memory Capacity , 2012, PloS one.

[33]  Michael W. L. Chee,et al.  Reduced functional segregation between the default mode network and the executive control network in healthy older adults: A longitudinal study , 2016, NeuroImage.

[34]  E. Vogel,et al.  Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex , 2004 .

[35]  Diego Pinal,et al.  Effects of load and maintenance duration on the time course of information encoding and retrieval in working memory: from perceptual analysis to post-categorization processes , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[36]  Paul M Bays,et al.  Dynamic Updating of Working Memory Resources for Visual Objects , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  FSL , 2012, NeuroImage.

[38]  N. Thakor,et al.  Quantitative EEG analysis methods and clinical applications , 2009 .

[39]  Daniel Brandeis,et al.  Frontal Midline Theta Reflects Individual Task Performance in a Working Memory Task , 2014, Brain Topography.

[40]  João Jorge,et al.  EEG–fMRI integration for the study of human brain function , 2014, NeuroImage.

[41]  Louis Lemieux,et al.  Identification of EEG Events in the MR Scanner: The Problem of Pulse Artifact and a Method for Its Subtraction , 1998, NeuroImage.

[42]  E. Vogel,et al.  Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.

[43]  Arno Villringer,et al.  Internal ventilation system of MR scanners induces specific EEG artifact during simultaneous EEG-fMRI , 2013, NeuroImage.

[44]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural activity predicts individual differences in visual working memory capacity , 2004, Nature.

[45]  Jan Derrfuss,et al.  Different Roles of Direct and Indirect Frontoparietal Pathways for Individual Working Memory Capacity , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  Keisuke Fukuda,et al.  α Power Modulation and Event-Related Slow Wave Provide Dissociable Correlates of Visual Working Memory , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  Sarit Ashkenazi,et al.  Visuo–spatial working memory is an important source of domain-general vulnerability in the development of arithmetic cognition , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[49]  R. Kessels,et al.  An exploratory study of the effects of spatial working-memory load on prefrontal activation in low- and high-performing elderly , 2014, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[50]  Meredith A. Shafto,et al.  Preserved cognitive functions with age are determined by domain-dependent shifts in network responsivity , 2017, Nature Communications.

[51]  M. Manosevitz High-Speed Scanning in Human Memory , .

[52]  Juan Zhou,et al.  Spontaneous eyelid closures link vigilance fluctuation with fMRI dynamic connectivity states , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[53]  E. Bramon,et al.  Spatial working memory in individuals at high risk for psychosis: Longitudinal fMRI study , 2010, Schizophrenia Research.

[54]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Differential coupling of visual cortex with default network or frontal-parietal network based on goals , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  N. Cowan,et al.  The Magical Mystery Four , 2010, Current directions in psychological science.

[56]  B. Biswal,et al.  Functional connectivity of default mode network components: Correlation, anticorrelation, and causality , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[57]  S. Makeig,et al.  Electroencephalography Correlates of Spatial Working Memory Deficits in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Vigilance, Encoding, and Maintenance , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  Evan M Gordon,et al.  Using spatial multiple regression to identify intrinsic connectivity networks involved in working memory performance , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[59]  Edward F. Ester,et al.  Parietal and Frontal Cortex Encode Stimulus-Specific Mnemonic Representations during Visual Working Memory , 2015, Neuron.

[60]  John T. Serences,et al.  Reconstructions of Information in Visual Spatial Working Memory Degrade with Memory Load , 2014, Current Biology.

[61]  Aiden E. G. F. Arnold,et al.  Intraparietal sulcus activity and functional connectivity supporting spatial working memory manipulation. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[62]  Andrew R. A. Conway,et al.  On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[63]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Neural Suppression of Irrelevant Information Underlies Optimal Working Memory Performance , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[64]  N. Cowan The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[65]  Robert Turner,et al.  A Method for Removing Imaging Artifact from Continuous EEG Recorded during Functional MRI , 2000, NeuroImage.

[66]  Maria Stein,et al.  A Tutorial on Data-Driven Methods for Statistically Assessing ERP Topographies , 2013, Brain Topography.

[67]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Performance level modulates adult age differences in brain activation during spatial working memory , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[68]  Christian Beste,et al.  Crosslinking EEG time–frequency decomposition and fMRI in error monitoring , 2013, Brain Structure and Function.

[69]  Raymond J Dolan,et al.  Maintenance versus manipulation in verbal working memory revisited: an fMRI study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[70]  J. Palva,et al.  Neuronal synchrony reveals working memory networks and predicts individual memory capacity , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[71]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[72]  Li Yao,et al.  Cortical activities of single-trial P300 amplitudes modulated by memory load using simultaneous EEG-fMRI , 2015, Medical Imaging.

[73]  R. Engle,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[74]  J. Matias Palva,et al.  Localization of Cortical Phase and Amplitude Dynamics during Visual Working Memory Encoding and Retention , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[75]  Robert T. Knight,et al.  Spatio-temporal dynamics of neural mechanisms underlying component operations in working memory , 2008, Brain Research.

[76]  D. Durstewitz,et al.  The Dual-State Theory of Prefrontal Cortex Dopamine Function with Relevance to Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Genotypes and Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[77]  E. Vogel,et al.  Visual working memory capacity: from psychophysics and neurobiology to individual differences , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[78]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Distributed and Dynamic Storage of Working Memory Stimulus Information in Extrastriate Cortex , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[79]  Edward K. Vogel,et al.  Event-Related Potential Measures of Visual Working Memory , 2006, Clinical EEG and neuroscience.

[80]  Giancarlo Valente,et al.  The Default Mode Network and the Working Memory Network Are Not Anti-Correlated during All Phases of a Working Memory Task , 2015, PloS one.

[81]  M. Bodner,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and sensory cortices during working memory: quantity and quality , 2015, Neuroscience Bulletin.

[82]  Rachael D. Seidler,et al.  Failure to Engage Spatial Working Memory Contributes to Age-related Declines in Visuomotor Learning , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.