Evaluating potential EEG-indicators for auditory attention to speech in realistic environmental noise

The human brain is remarkably capable of perceiving relevant sounds in noisy environments but the underlying interplay of neurophysiology and acoustics is still being investigated. Cortical processing of these sounds in the brain depends on attentional demand. One of the most important issues is how to identify whether a person is paying attention to the relevant sounds or not. The aim of this study was to explore the potential of single-trial electroencephalography (EEG) indicators to distinguish the cortical representation of three sequential tasks — attentive listening to lectures in background noise, attentive and inattentive listening to background noise alone. Three types of environmental noise, including multi-talker babble, fluctuating traffic and highway sounds were employed as the background during the first task and the stimulus during the second and third tasks. 23 healthy volunteers were exposed to these three tasks while 64-channels EEG signals were recorded. Alpha-band spectral characteristics (peak frequency and power) were investigated as potential indicators of attention and cortical inhibition. Furthermore, based on the hypothesis of self-similarity as excitation-inhibition balance, long-range temporal correlation of alpha-band activity was quantified based on detrended fluctuation analysis. Finally, the hypothesis of speech envelope entrainment of brain activity motivated to estimate the delta absolute power for investigating the attended sound. Considering the participant as a random factor, a linear mixed-effect regression was employed to model the estimated indicators as a function of listening task, EEG channel cluster, and background noise. Strong significant differences were found that support our hypotheses that auditory attention to speech can be observed via EEG-indicators.

[1]  N. Yeung,et al.  The roles of cortical oscillations in sustained attention , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[2]  M. Osaka,et al.  Peak alpha frequency of EEG during a mental task: task difficulty and hemispheric differences. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[3]  Antoine J. Shahin,et al.  Attentional Gain Control of Ongoing Cortical Speech Representations in a “Cocktail Party” , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  Axel Hutt,et al.  Dynamic Control of Synchronous Activity in Networks of Spiking Neurons , 2016, PloS one.

[5]  John Kounios,et al.  Peak alpha frequency: an electroencephalographic measure of cognitive preparedness , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[6]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[7]  Wolfgang Klimesch,et al.  State-dependent alpha peak frequency shifts: Experimental evidence, potential mechanisms and functional implications , 2017, Neuroscience.

[8]  Axel Hutt,et al.  Stimulus Statistics Shape Oscillations in Nonlinear Recurrent Neural Networks , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  Petre Stoica,et al.  Spectral Analysis of Signals , 2009 .

[10]  J. Lisman,et al.  Oscillations in the alpha band (9-12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  Arjen van Ooyen,et al.  Genetic Contributions to Long-Range Temporal Correlations in Ongoing Oscillations , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Arthur Jay Barabell,et al.  Improving the resolution performance of eigenstructure-based direction-finding algorithms , 1983, ICASSP.

[13]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[14]  Jonathan Z. Simon,et al.  Adaptive Temporal Encoding Leads to a Background-Insensitive Cortical Representation of Speech , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Mounya Elhilali,et al.  Modelling auditory attention , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[16]  T. Brismar,et al.  Long-range temporal correlations in electroencephalographic oscillations: Relation to topography, frequency band, age and gender , 2005, Neuroscience.

[17]  Nikulin Vadim,et al.  Detrended fluctuation analysis: a scale-free view on neuronal oscillations , 2012 .

[18]  K. Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Strong long‐range temporal correlations of beta/gamma oscillations are associated with poor sustained visual attention performance , 2017, The European journal of neuroscience.

[19]  Andero Uusberg,et al.  EEG alpha and cortical inhibition in affective attention. , 2013, International Journal of Psychophysiology.

[20]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[21]  C. Peng,et al.  Mosaic organization of DNA nucleotides. , 1994, Physical review. E, Statistical physics, plasmas, fluids, and related interdisciplinary topics.

[22]  K. Linkenkaer-Hansen,et al.  Neuronal long-range temporal correlations and avalanche dynamics are correlated with behavioral scaling laws , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[23]  Biyu J. He Scale-free brain activity: past, present, and future , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  W. Klimesch,et al.  EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition–timing hypothesis , 2007, Brain Research Reviews.

[25]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Alpha frequency, cognitive load and memory performance , 1993, Brain Topography.

[26]  Rufin VanRullen,et al.  Individual alpha peak frequency predicts 10 Hz flicker effects on selective attention , 2017 .