Contributions of metabolic and temporal costs to human gait selection

Humans naturally select several parameters within a gait that correspond with minimizing metabolic cost. Much less is understood about the role of metabolic cost in selecting between gaits. Here, we asked participants to decide between walking or running out and back to different gait specific markers. The distance of the walking marker was adjusted after each decision to identify relative distances where individuals switched gait preferences. We found that neither minimizing solely metabolic energy nor minimizing solely movement time could predict how the group decided between gaits. Of our twenty participants, six behaved in a way that tended towards minimizing metabolic energy, while eight favoured strategies that tended more towards minimizing movement time. The remaining six participants could not be explained by minimizing a single cost. We provide evidence that humans consider not just a single movement cost, but instead a weighted combination of these conflicting costs with their relative contributions varying across participants. Individuals who placed a higher relative value on time ran faster than individuals who placed a higher relative value on metabolic energy. Sensitivity to temporal costs also explained variability in an individual's preferred velocity as a function of increasing running distance. Interestingly, these differences in velocity both within and across participants were absent in walking, possibly due to a steeper metabolic cost of transport curve. We conclude that metabolic cost plays an essential, but not exclusive role in gait decisions.

[1]  Sergio A. Lambertucci,et al.  Energy Landscapes Shape Animal Movement Ecology , 2013, The American Naturalist.

[2]  Leroy L. Long,et al.  Walking, running, and resting under time, distance, and average speed constraints: optimality of walk–run–rest mixtures , 2013, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[3]  T. Garland,et al.  The biological control of voluntary exercise, spontaneous physical activity and daily energy expenditure in relation to obesity: human and rodent perspectives , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[4]  Helen J. Huang,et al.  A Representation of Effort in Decision-Making and Motor Control , 2016, Current Biology.

[5]  L. Green,et al.  Discounting of delayed rewards: Models of individual choice. , 1995, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[6]  Alaa A. Ahmed,et al.  Does risk-sensitivity transfer across movements? , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Karen L Steudel-Numbers,et al.  The evolution of human running: effects of changes in lower-limb length on locomotor economy. , 2007, Journal of human evolution.

[8]  M. O'Neill Gait-specific metabolic costs and preferred speeds in ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta), with implications for the scaling of locomotor costs. , 2012, American journal of physical anthropology.

[9]  J. E. Mazur An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. , 1987 .

[10]  Alaa A. Ahmed,et al.  Threat affects risk preferences in movement decision making , 2015, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[11]  Andrew E Kilding,et al.  Lower-Body Determinants of Running Economy in Male and Female Distance Runners , 2014, Journal of strength and conditioning research.

[12]  C. Pert,et al.  Enkephalins, catecholamines, and psychological mood alterations: effects of prolonged exercise. , 1987, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[13]  Alaa A. Ahmed,et al.  Rationality in Human Movement , 2016, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[14]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  The metabolic cost of changing walking speeds is significant, implies lower optimal speeds for shorter distances, and increases daily energy estimates , 2015, Biology Letters.

[15]  Cara M Wall-Scheffler,et al.  People choose to run at their optimal speed. , 2017, American journal of physical anthropology.

[16]  Rodolfo Margaria,et al.  Biomechanics and Energetics of Muscular Exercise , 1976 .

[17]  Jennie E. S. Choi,et al.  Vigor of Movements and the Cost of Time in Decision Making , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  D. F. Hoyt,et al.  Gait and the energetics of locomotion in horses , 1981, Nature.

[20]  B. Franklin,et al.  Updating ACSM's Recommendations for Exercise Preparticipation Health Screening. , 2015, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[21]  K. Hutchison,et al.  Cannabis and Exercise Science: A Commentary on Existing Studies and Suggestions for Future Directions , 2015, Sports Medicine.

[22]  Greta C Bernatz,et al.  How humans walk: bout duration, steps per bout, and rest duration. , 2008, Journal of rehabilitation research and development.

[23]  B. Galna,et al.  Free-living gait characteristics in ageing and Parkinson’s disease: impact of environment and ambulatory bout length , 2016, Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation.

[24]  P. Salmon,et al.  Effects of temporary withdrawal from regular running. , 1990, Journal of psychosomatic research.

[25]  Lionel Rigoux,et al.  A Model of Reward- and Effort-Based Optimal Decision Making and Motor Control , 2012, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[26]  Alena M. Grabowski,et al.  Does Metabolic Rate Increase Linearly with Running Speed in all Distance Runners? , 2017, Sports Medicine International Open.

[27]  Alaa A. Ahmed,et al.  Vigor of reaching movements: reward discounts the cost of effort. , 2018, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  Magellanic Penguins Spheniscus magellanicus commuting through san Julian bay; do current trends induce tidal tactics? , 2001 .

[29]  Carlo Capelli,et al.  Energetics of running in top-level marathon runners from Kenya , 2012, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[30]  Benjamin M. Ogles,et al.  A typology of marathon runners based on cluster analysis of motivations. , 2003 .

[31]  R. Kram,et al.  Effects of obesity and sex on the energetic cost and preferred speed of walking. , 2006, Journal of applied physiology.

[32]  K Schmidt-Nielsen,et al.  Locomotion: energy cost of swimming, flying, and running. , 1972, Science.

[33]  Alain Belli,et al.  Do mechanical gait parameters explain the higher metabolic cost of walking in obese adolescents? , 2009, Journal of applied physiology.

[34]  Graham H. Pyke,et al.  Optimal Foraging: A Selective Review of Theory and Tests , 1977, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[35]  Angelique G. Brellenthin,et al.  Endocannabinoid and Mood Responses to Exercise in Adults with Varying Activity Levels , 2017, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[36]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  To walk or to fly? How birds choose among foraging modes. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  Karen L Steudel-Numbers,et al.  Optimal running speed and the evolution of hominin hunting strategies. , 2009, Journal of human evolution.

[38]  H. Ralston Energy-speed relation and optimal speed during level walking , 1958, Internationale Zeitschrift für angewandte Physiologie einschließlich Arbeitsphysiologie.