The effect of a sequential structure of practice for the training of perceptual-cognitive skills in tennis

Objective Anticipation of opponent actions, through the use of advanced (i.e., pre-event) kinematic information, can be trained using video-based temporal occlusion. Typically, this involves isolated opponent skills/shots presented as trials in a random order. However, two different areas of research concerning representative task design and contextual (non-kinematic) information, suggest this structure of practice restricts expert performance. The aim of this study was to examine the effect of a sequential structure of practice during video-based training of anticipatory behavior in tennis, as well as the transfer of these skills to the performance environment. Methods In a pre-practice-retention-transfer design, participants viewed life-sized video of tennis rallies across practice in either a sequential order (sequential group), in which participants were exposed to opponent skills/shots in the order they occur in the sport, or a non-sequential (non-sequential group) random order. Results In the video-based retention test, the sequential group was significantly more accurate in their anticipatory judgments when the retention condition replicated the sequential structure compared to the non-sequential group. In the non-sequential retention condition, the non-sequential group was more accurate than the sequential group. In the field-based transfer test, overall decision time was significantly faster in the sequential group compared to the non-sequential group. Conclusion Findings highlight the benefits of a sequential structure of practice for the transfer of anticipatory behavior in tennis. We discuss the role of contextual information, and the importance of representative task design, for the testing and training of perceptual-cognitive skills in sport.

[1]  P. Ward,et al.  Domain Specificity, Task Specificity, and Expert Performance , 2008, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[2]  A. Williams,et al.  Training perceptual skill in sport. , 1999 .

[3]  F.R.H. Zijlstra,et al.  Efficiency in work behaviour: A design approach for modern tools , 1993 .

[4]  Colm P Murphy,et al.  Contextual information and perceptual-cognitive expertise in a dynamic, temporally-constrained task. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[5]  K. Davids,et al.  Examination of gaze behaviors under in situ and video simulation task constraints reveals differences in information pickup for perception and action , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[6]  Thomas A. Stoffregen,et al.  On the nature and evaluation of fidelity in virtual environments: Applications, Implications, and Human Performance , 2019 .

[7]  A. Williams,et al.  Anticipation skill in a real-world task: measurement, training, and transfer in tennis. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[8]  A Mark Williams,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: some considerations when applying the expert performance approach. , 2005, Human movement science.

[9]  G. Savelsbergh,et al.  Ventral and dorsal system contributions to visual anticipation in fast ball sports. , 2008 .

[10]  Paul R. Ford,et al.  Contextual interference effect on perceptual-cognitive skills training. , 2015, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[11]  Paul R. Ford,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport and its acquisition: implications for applied cognitive psychology , 2011 .

[12]  N. Hagemann,et al.  Pattern-induced expectation bias in visual anticipation of action outcomes. , 2015, Acta psychologica.

[13]  R. Cañal-Bruland,et al.  Action preferences and the anticipation of action outcomes. , 2014, Acta psychologica.

[14]  D. Farrow,et al.  The contribution of situational probability information to anticipatory skill. , 2012, Journal of science and medicine in sport.

[15]  Keith Davids,et al.  Movement Models from Sports Provide Representative Task Constraints for Studying Adaptive Behavior in Human Movement Systems , 2006, Adapt. Behav..

[16]  A. Williams,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive expertise in sport : implications for skill acquisition and performance enhancement , 2004 .

[17]  A. Williams,et al.  Anxiety, anticipation and contextual information: A test of attentional control theory , 2016, Cognition & emotion.

[18]  M. Guadagnoli,et al.  Challenge Point: A Framework for Conceptualizing the Effects of Various Practice Conditions in Motor Learning , 2004, Journal of motor behavior.

[19]  M. L. Barnett,et al.  Motor skills learning and the specificity of training principle. , 1973, Research quarterly.

[20]  Masahiro Shinya,et al.  A split-step shortens the time to perform a choice reaction step-and-reach movement in a simulated tennis task , 2009, Journal of sports sciences.

[21]  Paul R. Ford,et al.  Perceptual-cognitive skill training and its transfer to expert performance in the field: Future research directions , 2015, European journal of sport science.

[22]  B. Abernethy,et al.  Expertise and the Perception of Kinematic and Situational Probability Information , 2001, Perception.

[23]  A. Williams,et al.  Quantifying the nature of anticipation in professional tennis , 2013, Journal of sports sciences.

[24]  Paul Ward,et al.  The relative effectiveness of various instructional approaches in developing anticipation skill. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[25]  R. Cañal-Bruland,et al.  Time to broaden the scope of research on anticipatory behavior: a case for the role of probabilistic information , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[26]  Y. Fery,et al.  Effect of tactical initiative on predicting passing shots in tennis , 2005 .

[27]  N. Hagemann,et al.  Left-Handedness in Professional and Amateur Tennis , 2012, PloS one.

[28]  G Atkinson,et al.  Statistical Methods For Assessing Measurement Error (Reliability) in Variables Relevant to Sports Medicine , 1998, Sports medicine.

[29]  K. Davids,et al.  Ecological Validity, Representative Design, and Correspondence Between Experimental Task Constraints and Behavioral Setting: Comment on Rogers, Kadar, and Costall (2005) , 2007 .

[30]  K. Davids,et al.  Representative learning design and functionality of research and practice in sport. , 2011, Journal of sport & exercise psychology.

[31]  Paul R. Ford,et al.  Expertise and expert performance in sport , 2008 .

[32]  David W. Eccles,et al.  The effect of manipulating context-specific information on perceptual-cognitive processes during a simulated anticipation task. , 2011, British journal of psychology.

[33]  A. Williams Advance cue utilization in soccer , 1993 .