Echolocation intensity and directionality of perching and flying fringe-lipped bats, Trachops cirrhosus (Phyllostomidae)

The Neotropical frog-eating bat, Trachops cirrhosus, primarily hunts stationary prey, either by gleaning on the wing, or in a sit-and-wait mode hanging from a perch. It listens passively for prey-generated sounds, but uses echolocation in all stages of the hunt. Like other bats in the family Phyllostomidae, T. cirrhosus has a conspicuous nose leaf, hypothesized to direct and focus echolocation calls emitted from the nostrils. T. cirrhosus is highly flexible in its cognitive abilities and its use of sensory strategies for prey detection. Additionally, T. cirrhosus has been observed to echolocate both with closed and open mouth. We hypothesize that its flexibility extends to echolocation call design. We investigated the effect of hunting mode, perching or flying, as well as the effect of mouth opening, on the acoustic parameters and directionality of the echolocation call. We used a multi-microphone array, a high-speed video camera, and a microphone-diode-video system to directly visualize the echolocation sound beam synchronized with the bat's behavior. We found that T. cirrhosus emits a highly directional sound beam with half amplitude angle (HAM) of 12–18° and DI (directionality index) of ~17 dB, among the most directional bat sonar beams measured to date. The directionality was high both when flying and when perching. The emitted intensity was low, around 88 dB SPL at 10 cm from the mouth, when hanging, but higher, around 100 dB SPL at 10 cm, when flying or just before take-off. Our data suggests that the limited search volume of T. cirrhosus sonar beam defined by the high directionality and the rather low intensity of its echolocation calls is adapted to the highly cluttered hunting habitat and to the perch hunting mode.

[1]  Lasse Jakobsen,et al.  Vespertilionid bats control the width of their biosonar sound beam dynamically during prey pursuit , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  M. Ryan,et al.  Echolocation calls produced by Trachops cirrhosus (Chiroptera: Phyllostomatidae) while hunting for frogs , 1981 .

[3]  J. Speakman,et al.  No cost of echolocation for bats in flight , 1991, Nature.

[4]  E. Kalko,et al.  Dynamic adjustment of biosonar intensity to habitat clutter in the bat Macrophyllum macrophyllum (Phyllostomidae) , 2010, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[5]  E. Kalko,et al.  Sequential assessment of prey through the use of multiple sensory cues by an eavesdropping bat , 2012, Naturwissenschaften.

[6]  A. Surlykke,et al.  Auditory Relationships to Size in Noctuid Moths: Bigger Is Better , 1999, Naturwissenschaften.

[7]  Madeline Helland,et al.  Field Guide , 2020, Desert Weeds.

[8]  Perch-hunting in insectivorous Rhinolophus bats is related to the high energy costs of manoeuvring in flight , 2010, Journal of Comparative Physiology B.

[9]  Rolf Müller,et al.  Noseleaf Dynamics during Pulse Emission in Horseshoe Bats , 2012, PloS one.

[10]  E. Kalko,et al.  Echolocation call intensity and directionality in flying short-tailed fruit bats, Carollia perspicillata (Phyllostomidae). , 2011, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  H. Peremans,et al.  What Noseleaves Do for FM Bats Depends on Their Degree of Sensorial Specialization , 2010, PloS one.

[12]  H. Schnitzler [Compensation of Doppler effects in horseshoe bats]. , 1967, Die Naturwissenschaften.

[13]  Jeremy M. V. Rayner,et al.  Foraging behavior and echolocation of wild horseshoe bats Rhinolophus ferrumequinum and R. hipposideros (Chiroptera, Rhinolophidae) , 1989, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[14]  D. Kramer Foraging Behavior , 2022 .

[15]  G. Marimuthu,et al.  Foraging behavior of the indian false vampire bat, Megaderma lyra (Chiroptera: Megadermatidae) , 1991 .

[16]  Glenis R. Long,et al.  Masked auditory thresholds from the bat,Rhinolophus ferrumequinum , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[17]  M. Ryan,et al.  Behavioral responses of the frog-eating bat,Trachops cirrhosus, to sonic frequencies , 1983, Journal of comparative physiology.

[18]  E. Kalko,et al.  Ecological niche and phylogeny: the highly complex echolocation behavior of the trawling long-legged bat, Macrophyllum macrophyllum , 2007, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[19]  M. Fenton,et al.  Foraging and habitat use by Nycteris grandis (Chiroptera: Nycteridae) in Zimbabwe , 1987 .

[20]  G. Neuweiler,et al.  Foraging ecology and audition in echolocating bats. , 1989, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[21]  P. Madsen,et al.  The monopulsed nature of sperm whale clicks. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[22]  M. Ryan,et al.  Bat Predation and the Evolution of Frog Vocalizations in the Neotropics , 1981, Science.

[23]  Surlykke Annemarie Convergent Acoustic Field of View in Echolocating Bats , 2012 .

[24]  C. Moss,et al.  Adaptive vocal behavior drives perception by echolocation in bats , 2011, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[25]  Lasse Jakobsen,et al.  Echolocating bats emit a highly directional sonar sound beam in the field , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[26]  Echolocation beam shape in emballonurid bats, Saccopteryx bilineata and Cormura brevirostris , 2012, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[27]  D. Hartley,et al.  The sound emission pattern and the acoustical role of the noseleaf in the echolocating bat, Carollia perspicillata. , 1987, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[28]  M. Ryan,et al.  Bat Predation and Sexual Advertisement in a Neotropical Anuran , 1982, The American Naturalist.

[29]  M. Holderied,et al.  An Aerial-Hawking Bat Uses Stealth Echolocation to Counter Moth Hearing , 2010, Current Biology.

[30]  Hans-Ulrich Schnitzler,et al.  Roosting and Foraging Behavior of Two Neotropical Gleaning Bats, Tonatia silvicola and Trachops cirrhosus (Phyllostomidae) 1 , 1999 .

[31]  C. Moss,et al.  Acoustic scanning of natural scenes by echolocation in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[32]  M. Ryan,et al.  The effect of signal complexity on localization performance in bats that localize frog calls , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[33]  E. Kalko,et al.  MORPHOLOGICAL CHARACTERISTICS PREDICT ALTERNATE FORAGING STRATEGY AND MICROHABITAT SELECTION IN THE ORANGE-BELLIED BAT, LAMPRONYCTERIS BRACHYOTIS , 2004 .

[34]  R. Rübsamen,et al.  Foraging behaviour and echolocation in the rufous horseshoe bat (Rhinolophus rouxi) of Sri Lanka , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[35]  E. Kalko,et al.  Echolocating Bats Cry Out Loud to Detect Their Prey , 2008, PloS one.

[36]  H. Schnitzler,et al.  Echolocation by Insect-Eating Bats , 2001 .

[37]  B. Kirkpatrick,et al.  Wildlife of the Tibetan Steppes , 2000 .

[38]  Frank Borina,et al.  Psychophysical and neurophysiological hearing thresholds in the bat Phyllostomus discolor , 2007, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.