Early and late evoked brain responses differentially reflect feature encoding and perception in the flash-lag illusion

In the flash-lag illusion (FLI), the position of a flash presented ahead of a moving bar is mislocalized, so the flash appears to lag the bar. Currently, it is not clear whether this effect is due to early perceptual-related neural processes such as motion extrapolation or reentrant processing, or due to later feedback processing relating to postdiction, i.e., retroactively altered perception. We presented 17 participants with the FLI paradigm while recording EEG. A central flash occurred either 51ms ("early") or 16ms ("late") before the bar moving from left to right reached the screen center. Participants judged whether the flash appeared to the right ("no flash lag illusion") or to the left ("flash-lag illusion") of the bar. Using single-trial linear modelling, we examined the influence of timing ("early" vs. "late") and perception ("illusion" vs. "no illusion") on flash-evoked brain responses and estimated the cortical sources underlying the FLI. An earlier frontal and occipital component (200-276ms) differentiated time-locked early vs. late stimulus presentation, indicating that early evoked brain responses reflect feature encoding in the FLI. Perception of the FLI was associated with a late window (368-452ms) in the ERP, with larger deflections for illusion than no illusion trials, localized to the left inferior occipital gyrus. This suggests a postdiction-related reconstruction of ambiguous sensory stimulation involving late processes in the occipito-temporal cortex, previously associated with temporal integration phenomena. Our findings indicate that perception of the FLI relies on an interplay between ongoing stimulus encoding of the moving bar and feedback processing of the flash, which takes place at later integration stages.

[1]  R. VanRullen,et al.  Conscious updating is a rhythmic process , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Postdiction: its implications on visual awareness, hindsight, and sense of agency , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[3]  S. Klein,et al.  Evidence for an Attentional Component of the Perceptual Misalignment between Moving and Flashing Stimuli , 2002, Perception.

[4]  J. Vroomen,et al.  An event-related potential investigation of the time-course of temporal ventriloquism , 2005, Neuroreport.

[5]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[6]  Matthias L. Schroeter,et al.  Identifying neural correlates of visual consciousness with ALE meta-analyses , 2015, NeuroImage.

[7]  N. Ramsey,et al.  Predictive coding for motion stimuli in human early visual cortex , 2016, Brain Structure and Function.

[8]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Digital filter design for electrophysiological data – a practical approach , 2015, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[9]  Romi Nijhawan,et al.  Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.

[10]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. , 2000, Science.

[11]  C. Sergent The offline stream of conscious representations , 2018, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[12]  Hinze Hogendoorn,et al.  Working Title: Motion Extrapolation in Visual Processing: Lessons from 25 Years of Flash-Lag Debate , 2020 .

[13]  W. Drongelen,et al.  Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering , 1997, IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering.

[14]  F. Blankenburg,et al.  Dissociating Perceptual Awareness and Postperceptual Processing: The P300 Is Not a Reliable Marker of Somatosensory Target Detection , 2021, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[16]  Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al.  Masking Disrupts Reentrant Processing in Human Visual Cortex , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[19]  Hinze Hogendoorn,et al.  Predictive Coding with Neural Transmission Delays: A Real-Time Temporal Alignment Hypothesis , 2018, eNeuro.

[20]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[21]  Julian Keil,et al.  Temporal orienting precedes intersensory attention and has opposing effects on early evoked brain activity , 2017, NeuroImage.

[22]  V. Arstila Keeping postdiction simple , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[23]  W-J Huang,et al.  The neurophysiology of P 300--an integrated review. , 2015, European review for medical and pharmacological sciences.

[24]  M. Koivisto,et al.  ERP and MEG correlates of visual consciousness: The second decade , 2020, Consciousness and Cognition.

[25]  Terrence J Sejnowski,et al.  Motion signals bias localization judgments: a unified explanation for the flash-lag, flash-drag, flash-jump, and Frohlich illusions. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[26]  Anthony N. Burkitt,et al.  Predictive coding of visual object position ahead of moving objects revealed by time-resolved EEG decoding , 2018, NeuroImage.

[27]  B. Aczel,et al.  Estimating the evidential value of significant results in psychological science , 2017, PloS one.

[28]  I. Toni,et al.  Oscillations , 2018, Physics to a Degree.

[29]  Florent Meyniel,et al.  The Neural Representation of Sequences: From Transition Probabilities to Algebraic Patterns and Linguistic Trees , 2015, Neuron.

[30]  Timothy L Hubbard,et al.  The flash-lag effect and related mislocalizations: findings, properties, and theories. , 2014, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  M. Chappell,et al.  Dividing attention in the flash-lag illusion , 2007, Vision Research.

[32]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Brain Dynamics Underlying the Nonlinear Threshold for Access to Consciousness , 2007, PLoS biology.

[33]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[34]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  Independent Component Analysis Using an Extended Infomax Algorithm for Mixed Subgaussian and Supergaussian Sources , 1999, Neural Computation.

[35]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion-Dependent Representation of Space in Area MT+ , 2013, Neuron.

[36]  C. Olson,et al.  Statistical learning of visual transitions in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.