Lay Epistemic Theory: The Motivational, Cognitive, and Social Aspects of Knowledge Formation

We review and integrate three separate research programs emanating from the theory of lay epistemics (Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay Epistemics and Human Knowledge: Cognitive and Motivational Bases. New York: Plenum). The need for cognitive closure is an epistemic motivation that propels knowledge formation and has widely ramifying consequences for individual, interpersonal, and group phenomena. The unimodel investigates the process of new knowledge formation from the ‘information given.’ The work on epistemic authority highlights the centrality of social source effects, including the self as a source, in human epistemic behavior. These three research programs examine facets of epistemic behavior that function interdependently to produce the knowledge that guides individual and social functioning.

[1]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Category accessibility as implicit memory , 1988 .

[2]  Y. Trope,et al.  Processing alternative explanations of behavior: correction or integration? , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  The Elaboration Continuum by Any Other Name Does Not Smell as Sweet , 1999 .

[4]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  The need for cognition. , 1982 .

[5]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[6]  A. Tversky,et al.  On the psychology of prediction , 1973 .

[7]  H. Kelley,et al.  Communication And Persuasion , 1953 .

[8]  H. Erb,et al.  Searching for commonalities in human judgement: The parametric unimodel and its dual mode alternatives , 2003 .

[9]  A. Raviv,et al.  Says Who?: Epistemic Authority Effects in Social Judgment , 2005 .

[10]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  Exceptions that prove the rule: Using a theory of motivated social cognition to account for ideological incongruities and political anomalies , 2003 .

[11]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  The illusory second mode or, the cue is the message. , 1999 .

[12]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Generality of the effects of practice on social judgment tasks. , 1988 .

[13]  E. Higgins Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. , 1996 .

[14]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Assimilative behavior identification can also be resource dependent: the unimodel perspective on personal-attribution phases. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  Eliot R. Smith Model of social inference processes , 1984 .

[16]  Eliot R. Smith Procedural efficiency: General and specific components and effects on social judgment , 1989 .

[17]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[18]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[19]  A. Raviv,et al.  Development in children's perceptions of epistemic authorities* , 1990 .

[20]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  The social cognition of immigrants' acculturation: effects of the need for closure and the reference group at entry. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Arie W Kruglanski,et al.  Groups as epistemic providers: need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. , 2006, Psychological review.

[22]  H. Erb,et al.  Informational length and order of presentation as determinants of persuasion , 2005 .

[23]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Persuasion by a Single Route: A View From the Unimodel , 1999 .

[24]  Alice H. Eagly,et al.  Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. , 1989 .

[25]  J. Bruner Beyond the Information Given: Studies in the Psychology of Knowing , 1973 .

[26]  A. Kruglanski The Psychology of Closed Mindedness , 2004 .

[27]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[28]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. , 1999 .

[29]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Partitioning the domain of social inference: dual mode and systems models and their alternatives. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[30]  P. Tetlock Accountability: A social check on the fundamental attribution error. , 1985 .

[31]  Y. Trope,et al.  Effortfulness and flexibility of dispositional judgment processes. , 1997 .

[32]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  On The Parameters of Human Judgment , 2007 .

[33]  Curtis D. Hardin,et al.  Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjective objective. , 1996 .

[34]  Kristen M. Klein,et al.  Persistence of attitude change and attitude–behavior correspondence based on extensive processing of source information , 2012 .

[35]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Motivated Cognition and Group Interaction: Need for Closure Affects the Contents and Processes of Collective Negotiations , 1999 .

[36]  Shelly Chaiken,et al.  The heuristic-systematic model in its broader context. , 1999 .

[37]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Group members' reactions to opinion deviates and conformists at varying degrees of proximity to decision deadline and of environmental noise. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[38]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Membership has its (epistemic) rewards: need for closure effects on in-group bias. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[39]  Arie W. Kruglanski,et al.  What Makes You So Sure? Effects of Epistemic Motivations on judgmental Confidence , 1987 .

[40]  M. Kendall,et al.  The Logic of Scientific Discovery. , 1959 .

[41]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[42]  J. Jost,et al.  Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  M. Rokeach The effect of perception time upon rigidity and concreteness of thinking. , 1950, Journal of experimental psychology.

[44]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  When similarity breeds content: need for closure and the allure of homogeneous and self-resembling groups. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[45]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Autocracy Bias in Informal Groups Under Need for Closure , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[46]  T. Adorno The Authoritarian Personality , 1950 .

[47]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Relevance override: on the reduced impact of "cues" under high-motivation conditions of persuasion studies. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  The role of task demands and processing resources in the use of base-rate and individuating information. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[49]  Gary James Jason,et al.  The Logic of Scientific Discovery , 1988 .

[50]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring ☆ , 1983 .