Lay Epistemic Theory: The Motivational, Cognitive, and Social Aspects of Knowledge Formation
暂无分享,去创建一个
Arie W. Kruglanski | Antonio Pierro | A. Kruglanski | A. Pierro | M. Dechesne | E. Orehek | Edward Orehek | Mark Dechesne | Antonio Pierro
[1] Eliot R. Smith,et al. Category accessibility as implicit memory , 1988 .
[2] Y. Trope,et al. Processing alternative explanations of behavior: correction or integration? , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[3] Duane T. Wegener,et al. The Elaboration Continuum by Any Other Name Does Not Smell as Sweet , 1999 .
[4] J. Cacioppo,et al. The need for cognition. , 1982 .
[5] S. Chaiken,et al. Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .
[6] A. Tversky,et al. On the psychology of prediction , 1973 .
[7] H. Kelley,et al. Communication And Persuasion , 1953 .
[8] H. Erb,et al. Searching for commonalities in human judgement: The parametric unimodel and its dual mode alternatives , 2003 .
[9] A. Raviv,et al. Says Who?: Epistemic Authority Effects in Social Judgment , 2005 .
[10] Arie W. Kruglanski,et al. Exceptions that prove the rule: Using a theory of motivated social cognition to account for ideological incongruities and political anomalies , 2003 .
[11] A. Kruglanski,et al. The illusory second mode or, the cue is the message. , 1999 .
[12] Eliot R. Smith,et al. Generality of the effects of practice on social judgment tasks. , 1988 .
[13] E. Higgins. Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. , 1996 .
[14] A. Kruglanski,et al. Assimilative behavior identification can also be resource dependent: the unimodel perspective on personal-attribution phases. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[15] Eliot R. Smith. Model of social inference processes , 1984 .
[16] Eliot R. Smith. Procedural efficiency: General and specific components and effects on social judgment , 1989 .
[17] D. Spalding. The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.
[18] A. Kruglanski,et al. Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[19] A. Raviv,et al. Development in children's perceptions of epistemic authorities* , 1990 .
[20] A. Kruglanski,et al. The social cognition of immigrants' acculturation: effects of the need for closure and the reference group at entry. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[21] Arie W Kruglanski,et al. Groups as epistemic providers: need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. , 2006, Psychological review.
[22] H. Erb,et al. Informational length and order of presentation as determinants of persuasion , 2005 .
[23] A. Kruglanski,et al. Persuasion by a Single Route: A View From the Unimodel , 1999 .
[24] Alice H. Eagly,et al. Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. , 1989 .
[25] J. Bruner. Beyond the Information Given: Studies in the Psychology of Knowing , 1973 .
[26] A. Kruglanski. The Psychology of Closed Mindedness , 2004 .
[27] E. Higgins,et al. Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .
[28] Duane T. Wegener,et al. The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. , 1999 .
[29] A. Kruglanski,et al. Partitioning the domain of social inference: dual mode and systems models and their alternatives. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[30] P. Tetlock. Accountability: A social check on the fundamental attribution error. , 1985 .
[31] Y. Trope,et al. Effortfulness and flexibility of dispositional judgment processes. , 1997 .
[32] Arie W. Kruglanski,et al. On The Parameters of Human Judgment , 2007 .
[33] Curtis D. Hardin,et al. Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjective objective. , 1996 .
[34] Kristen M. Klein,et al. Persistence of attitude change and attitude–behavior correspondence based on extensive processing of source information , 2012 .
[35] A. Kruglanski,et al. Motivated Cognition and Group Interaction: Need for Closure Affects the Contents and Processes of Collective Negotiations , 1999 .
[36] Shelly Chaiken,et al. The heuristic-systematic model in its broader context. , 1999 .
[37] A. Kruglanski,et al. Group members' reactions to opinion deviates and conformists at varying degrees of proximity to decision deadline and of environmental noise. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[38] A. Kruglanski,et al. Membership has its (epistemic) rewards: need for closure effects on in-group bias. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[39] Arie W. Kruglanski,et al. What Makes You So Sure? Effects of Epistemic Motivations on judgmental Confidence , 1987 .
[40] M. Kendall,et al. The Logic of Scientific Discovery. , 1959 .
[41] John T. Cacioppo,et al. The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.
[42] J. Jost,et al. Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.
[43] M. Rokeach. The effect of perception time upon rigidity and concreteness of thinking. , 1950, Journal of experimental psychology.
[44] A. Kruglanski,et al. When similarity breeds content: need for closure and the allure of homogeneous and self-resembling groups. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[45] A. Kruglanski,et al. Autocracy Bias in Informal Groups Under Need for Closure , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[46] T. Adorno. The Authoritarian Personality , 1950 .
[47] A. Kruglanski,et al. Relevance override: on the reduced impact of "cues" under high-motivation conditions of persuasion studies. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[48] A. Kruglanski,et al. The role of task demands and processing resources in the use of base-rate and individuating information. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[49] Gary James Jason,et al. The Logic of Scientific Discovery , 1988 .
[50] A. Kruglanski,et al. The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring ☆ , 1983 .