The development of grasping comprehension in infancy: covert shifts of attention caused by referential actions

An eye tracking paradigm was used to investigate how infants’ attention is modulated by observed goal-directed manual grasping actions. In Experiment 1, we presented 3-, 5-, and 7-month-old infants with a static picture of a grasping hand, followed by a target appearing at a location either congruent or incongruent with the grasping direction of the hand. The latency of infants gaze shift from the hand to the target was recorded and compared between congruent and incongruent trials. Results demonstrate a congruency effect from 5 months of age. A second experiment illustrated that the congruency effect of Experiment 1 does not extend to a visually similar mechanical claw (instead of the grasping hand). Together these two experiments describe the onset of covert attention shifts in response to manual actions and relate these findings to the onset of manual grasping.

[1]  J. Sommerville,et al.  Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions , 2005, Cognition.

[2]  C. Hofsten,et al.  The development of reactive saccade latencies , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[4]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Infants predict other people's action goals , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[5]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[6]  E. Bullmore,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of implicit-motion perception or ‘representational momentum’ , 2000, Current Biology.

[7]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Eye Tracking in Infancy Research , 2009, Developmental neuropsychology.

[8]  A. Premack,et al.  Causal cognition : a multidisciplinary debate , 1996 .

[9]  Moritz M. Daum,et al.  The development and neural basis of pointing comprehension , 2010, Social neuroscience.

[10]  J. J. Guajardo,et al.  Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action , 2002 .

[11]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  When the ordinary seems unexpected: evidence for incremental physical knowledge in young infants , 2005, Cognition.

[12]  M. Candidi,et al.  Mapping Implied Body Actions in the Human Motor System , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  M. Legerstee,et al.  Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: understanding people and their actions. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[14]  K. Snow,et al.  Information processing through the first year of life: a longitudinal study using the visual expectation paradigm. , 2000, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[15]  H. Wellman,et al.  Infants' ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action , 2002, Cognition.

[16]  M. Tomasello,et al.  A new look at infant pointing. , 2007, Child development.

[17]  Claes von Hofsten,et al.  Developmental asymmetries between horizontal and vertical tracking , 2006, Vision Research.

[18]  Uta Frith,et al.  Your Own Action Influences How You Perceive Another Person's Action , 2004, Current Biology.

[19]  Christian Keysers,et al.  The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[20]  Pär Nyström,et al.  The infant mirror neuron system studied with high density EEG , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[21]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Activation in Human MT/MST by Static Images with Implied Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[22]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Observations on the development of reaching for moving objects. , 1979, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  R. Johansson,et al.  Action plans used in action observation , 2003, Nature.

[24]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Orienting of Attention , 2008 .

[25]  Olga Kochukhova,et al.  Preverbal infants anticipate that food will be brought to the mouth: an eye tracking study of manual feeding and flying spoons. , 2010, Child development.

[26]  G. Gredebäck,et al.  Infants’ understanding of everyday social interactions: A dual process account , 2010, Cognition.

[27]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Inferring the size of a goal object from an actor's grasping movement in 6- and 9-month-old infants. , 2009, Developmental science.

[28]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Predictive action in infancy: tracking and reaching for moving objects , 1998, Cognition.

[29]  G. Orban,et al.  Specificity of regions processing biological motion , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[30]  C. Johnson,et al.  Infant growth and development. , 1997, Pediatrics in review.

[31]  C. Von Hofsten,et al.  Action type and goal type modulate goal-directed gaze shifts in 14-month-old infants. , 2009, Developmental psychology.

[32]  R. Kail,et al.  Processing Time Declines Exponentially during Childhood and Adolescence. , 1991 .

[33]  S. Gelman,et al.  Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .

[34]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  A. Leslie A theory of agency. , 1995 .

[36]  D. Premack The infant's theory of self-propelled objects , 1990, Cognition.

[37]  G. Bronson,et al.  The Scanning Patterns of Human Infants: Implications for Visual Learning , 1982 .

[38]  Hein T. van Schie,et al.  You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy , 2008, NeuroImage.

[39]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Encyclopedia of the Human Brain , 2002 .

[40]  C. Hofsten An action perspective on motor development , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[41]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  C. Hofsten Eye–hand coordination in the newborn. , 1982 .

[43]  W. Prinz A common-coding approach to perception and action , 1990 .

[44]  Moritz M. Daum,et al.  Encoding the goal of an object-directed but uncompleted reaching action in 6- and 9-month-old infants. , 2008, Developmental science.

[45]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Eye contact detection in humans from birth , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[46]  A. Kingstone,et al.  The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze , 1998 .

[47]  Gisa Aschersleben,et al.  Infant's perception of goal-directed actions performed by a mechanical device , 2005 .

[48]  R. Langdon,et al.  Spatial cueing by social versus nonsocial directional signals , 2005 .

[49]  L. Cosmides,et al.  No interpretation without representation: the role of domain-specific representations and inferences in the Wason selection task , 2000, Cognition.

[50]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: a study of imitation. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  Jon Driver,et al.  The positive and negative of human expertise in gaze perception , 2000, Cognition.

[52]  R. E Passingham,et al.  Inferring false beliefs from the actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[53]  A. Woodward,et al.  Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others' goals. , 2008, Developmental science.

[54]  Jessica A. Sommerville,et al.  Infants' Sensitivity to the Causal Features of Means‐End Support Sequences in Action and Perception , 2005 .

[55]  Yuyan Luo,et al.  The attribution of attention: 9-month-olds' interpretation of gaze as goal-directed action. , 2007, Developmental science.

[56]  C. Heyes,et al.  Robotic movement elicits automatic imitation. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[57]  T Moore,et al.  Control of eye movements and spatial attention. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[58]  Jon Driver,et al.  Adult's Eyes Trigger Shifts of Visual Attention in Human Infants , 1998 .

[59]  R. Baillargeon,et al.  Detecting continuity violations in infancy: a new account and new evidence from covering and tube events , 2005, Cognition.

[60]  W. Prinz,et al.  Relationships Between Perception and Action: Current Approaches , 1990 .

[61]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Gaze Perception Triggers Reflexive Visuospatial Orienting , 1999 .

[62]  A. Leslie Mapping the mind: ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity , 1994 .

[63]  A. Woodward Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach , 1998, Cognition.

[64]  Jon Driver,et al.  Adult's eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants , 1998 .

[65]  A. Leslie,et al.  Infants' perception of goal-directed actions: development through cue-based bootstrapping. , 2007, Developmental science.

[66]  B. Repp,et al.  Action Can Affect Auditory Perception , 2007, Psychological science.

[67]  C. Urgesi,et al.  Simulating the future of actions in the human corticospinal system. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[68]  Amanda L. Woodward,et al.  Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object , 2003 .

[69]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Gaze Following in Newborns , 2004 .

[70]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Components of Visual Orienting in Early Infancy: Contingency Learning, Anticipatory Looking, and Disengaging , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[71]  Claes von Hofsten,et al.  Occlusion Is Hard: Comparing Predictive Reaching for Visible and Hidden Objects in Infants and Adults , 2009, Cogn. Sci..

[72]  C. Hofsten,et al.  Preparation for grasping an object: a developmental study. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[73]  J. Reznick,et al.  The development of visual expectations in the first year. , 2000, Child development.