Cognitive conflict in social dilemmas: An analysis of response dynamics

Recently, it has been suggested that people are spontaneously inclined to cooperate in social dilemmas, whereas defection requires effortful deliberation. From this assumption, we derive that defection should entail more cognitive conflict than cooperation. To test this hypothesis, the current study presents a first application of the response dynamics paradigm (i.e., mouse-tracking) to social dilemmas. In a fully incentivized lab experiment, mouse movements were tracked while participants played simple two-person social dilemma games with two options (cooperation and defection). Building on previous research, curvature of mouse movements was taken as an indicator of cognitive conflict. In line with the hypothesis of less cognitive conflict in cooperation, response trajectories were more curved (towards the non-chosen option) when individuals defected than when they cooperated. In other words, the cooperative option exerted more ``pull'' on mouse movements in case of defection than the non-cooperative option (defection) did in case of cooperation. This effect was robust across different types of social dilemmas and occurred even in the prisoner's dilemma, where defection was predominant on the choice level. Additionally, the effect was stronger for dispositional cooperators as measured by the Honesty-Humility factor of the HEXACO personality model. As such, variation in the effect across individuals could be accounted for through cooperativeness.

[1]  N. Chater,et al.  Game relativity: how context influences strategic decision making. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  G. Gigerenzer Gut Feelings: The Intelligence of the Unconscious , 2007 .

[3]  David G. Rand,et al.  Social Context and the Dynamics of Cooperative Choice , 2014 .

[4]  Valerie F. Reyna,et al.  Educating Intuition , 2015, Current directions in psychological science.

[5]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[6]  B. Hilbig,et al.  Pillars of cooperation: Honesty-Humility, social value orientations, and economic behavior , 2009 .

[7]  Ryan O. Murphy,et al.  Reciprocity as an Individual Difference , 2016 .

[8]  Jochen Musch,et al.  Defection in the dark? A randomized-response investigation of cooperativeness in social dilemma games , 2011 .

[9]  Gregory J. Koop An assessment of the temporal dynamics of moral decisions , 2013, Judgment and Decision Making.

[10]  P. Lang International affective picture system (IAPS) : affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual , 2005 .

[11]  David G. Rand,et al.  Reflection does not undermine self-interested prosociality , 2014, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[12]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Intuitive Prosociality , 2013 .

[13]  Joseph G. Johnson,et al.  Response dynamics: A new window on the decision process , 2011, Judgment and Decision Making.

[14]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  Action Dynamics Reveal Parallel Competition in Decision Making , 2008, Psychological science.

[15]  Daniel M. Oppenheimer,et al.  Heuristics made easy: an effort-reduction framework. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  M. Ashton,et al.  The HEXACO–60: A Short Measure of the Major Dimensions of Personality , 2009, Journal of personality assessment.

[17]  Guido Ortona,et al.  Is cooperation instinctive? Evidence from the response times in a public goods game , 2011 .

[18]  Yves Rosseel,et al.  lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling , 2012 .

[19]  P. V. Lange,et al.  The psychology of social dilemmas: A review. , 2013 .

[20]  S. Gächter Behavioral Game Theory , 2008, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[21]  Andreas Glöckner,et al.  Social Value Orientation and information search in social dilemmas: An eye-tracking analysis , 2013 .

[22]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  Continuous Dynamics in Real-Time Cognition , 2006 .

[23]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Surrogates for Theories , 1998 .

[24]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[25]  Michael Kahana,et al.  Unpacking Intuition: A Conjecture , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[26]  N. Ambady,et al.  Motions of the Hand Expose the Partial and Parallel Activation of Stereotypes , 2009, Psychological science.

[27]  A. Rapoport,et al.  The Game of Chicken , 1966 .

[28]  T. Goschke,et al.  How decisions emerge: action dynamics in intertemporal decision making. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[30]  A. Dijksterhuis,et al.  A Theory of Unconscious Thought , 2006, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[31]  B. Hilbig,et al.  Two sides of one coin: Honesty–Humility and situational factors mutually shape social dilemma decision making , 2013 .

[32]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  The Continuity Of Mind , 2008 .

[33]  Reaction times and reflection in social dilemmas: Extreme responses are fast, but not intuitive , 2014 .

[34]  P. Kollock SOCIAL DILEMMAS: The Anatomy of Cooperation , 1998 .

[35]  Rick Dale,et al.  The Self-Organization of Explicit Attitudes , 2009, Psychological science.

[36]  B. Hilbig,et al.  Personality, Punishment and Public Goods: Strategic Shifts towards Cooperation as a Matter of Dispositional Honesty–Humility , 2012 .

[37]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  Continuous attraction toward phonological competitors. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  B. Hilbig,et al.  Faktorenstruktur, psychometrische Eigenschaften und Messinvarianz der deutschsprachigen Version des 60- Item HEXACO Persönlichkeitsinventars , 2014 .

[39]  A. Glöckner,et al.  Personality and prosocial behavior: linking basic traits and social value orientations. , 2014, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[40]  D. Bates,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .

[41]  Benjamin E. Hilbig,et al.  Think or blink — is the recognition heuristic an “intuitive” strategy? , 2010, Judgment and Decision Making.

[42]  Daniel Västfjäll,et al.  Intuition and cooperation reconsidered , 2013, Nature.

[43]  C. McClintock,et al.  Social value related response latencies: Unobtrusive evidence for individual differences in information processing , 1993 .

[44]  L. Felkins The Social Dilemmas , 2015 .

[45]  Henrik Singmann,et al.  afex – Analysis of Factorial EXperiments , 2015 .

[46]  M. Johannesson,et al.  Rand et al. reply , 2013, Nature.

[47]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.

[48]  Wim B. G. Liebrand,et al.  The ring measure of social values: A computerized procedure for assessing individual differences in information processing and social value orientation , 1988 .

[49]  M. Ashton,et al.  The HEXACO Honesty-Humility, Agreeableness, and Emotionality Factors , 2014, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[50]  Andreas Glöckner,et al.  How Distinct are Intuition and Deliberation? An Eye-Tracking Analysis of Instruction-Induced Decision Modes , 2009, Judgment and Decision Making.

[51]  A. Rapoport,et al.  Prisoner's Dilemma: A Study in Conflict and Co-operation , 1970 .

[52]  Peter P. J. L. Verkoeijen,et al.  Does Intuition Cause Cooperation? , 2014, PloS one.

[53]  Jean-Robert Tyran,et al.  Second Thoughts on Free Riding , 2013 .

[54]  Erik Wengström,et al.  Fast or Fair? A Study of Response Times , 2008 .

[55]  G. Keren,et al.  Two Is Not Always Better Than One , 2009, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[56]  J. Baron Looking at Individual Subjects in Research on Judgment and Decision Making (or Anything) , 2009 .

[57]  David G. Rand,et al.  The Collective Benefits of Feeling Good and Letting Go: Positive Emotion and (dis)Inhibition Interact to Predict Cooperative Behavior , 2014, PloS one.

[58]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[59]  A. Glöckner,et al.  Beyond dual-process models: A categorisation of processes underlying intuitive judgement and decision making , 2010 .

[60]  Andreas Glöckner,et al.  Intuition in Judgment and Decision Making: Extensive Thinking Without Effort , 2010 .

[61]  David G. Rand,et al.  Spontaneous giving and calculated greed , 2012, Nature.

[62]  D. Kahneman Thinking, Fast and Slow , 2011 .

[63]  Thomas A. Farmer,et al.  Hand in Motion Reveals Mind in Motion , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[64]  Joseph G. Johnson,et al.  The response dynamics of preferential choice , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.

[65]  Benjamin E. Hilbig,et al.  It takes two : Honesty-humility and agreeableness differentially predict active versus reactive cooperation , 2013 .

[66]  Jonathan B Freeman,et al.  MouseTracker: Software for studying real-time mental processing using a computer mouse-tracking method , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[67]  David G. Rand,et al.  Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation , 2014, Nature Communications.

[68]  M. Ashton,et al.  Empirical, Theoretical, and Practical Advantages of the HEXACO Model of Personality Structure , 2007, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[69]  E-Step Structural Topic Models for Open Ended Survey Responses , 2022 .