Perceptual Expertise: Bridging Brain and Behavior
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] T. R. Miles,et al. Dyslexia research and its applications to education , 1981 .
[2] Thomas Sanocki. Intra- and interpattern relations in letter recognition. , 1991 .
[3] T. Sanocki,et al. Effects of early common features on form perception , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[4] G. Humphreys,et al. The Use of Abstract Graphemic Information in Lexical Access , 1981 .
[5] J. Hanley,et al. Preserved access to abstract letter identities despite abolished letter naming in a case of pure alexia , 2002, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[6] T. Allison,et al. Human extrastriate visual cortex and the perception of faces, words, numbers, and colors. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.
[7] Isabel Gauthier,et al. Letter processing in the visual system: Different activation patterns for single letters and strings , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[8] H. Coslett,et al. IMPLICIT VS. LETTER-BY-LETTER READING IN PURE ALEXIA: A TALE OF TWO SYSTEMS. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[9] Bernard Mazoyer,et al. Word and non-word reading: What role for the Visual Word Form Area? , 2005, NeuroImage.
[10] D. Perani,et al. The neural correlates of verb and noun processing. A PET study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[11] M. Coltheart. Disorders of reading and their implications for models of normal reading. , 1981 .
[12] J. Bowey,et al. Orthographic onsets and rimes as functional units of reading , 1990, Memory & cognition.
[13] T Shallice,et al. Word-form dyslexia. , 1980, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[14] D. D. Wheeler. Processes in word recognition , 1970 .
[15] L. Katz,et al. Cerebral organization of component processes in reading. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[16] J B Poline,et al. Letter Binding and Invariant Recognition of Masked Words , 2004, Psychological science.
[17] A. Ishai,et al. Distributed and Overlapping Representations of Faces and Objects in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2001, Science.
[18] H. Marmurek. Evidence against the computation of abstract letter identities in visual processing. , 1985, Canadian journal of psychology.
[19] Russell A. Epstein,et al. The Parahippocampal Place Area Recognition, Navigation, or Encoding? , 1999, Neuron.
[20] Bruce D. McCandliss,et al. The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[21] Edward E. Smith,et al. The role of syllables in perceptual processing , 1973 .
[22] G. E. MacKinnon,et al. Reading Research Advances in Theory and Practice , 1985 .
[23] I. Gauthier,et al. Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[24] T. Shallice,et al. Pure Alexia: A Nonspatial Visual Disorder Affecting Letter Activation , 1995 .
[25] T. Busey,et al. Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for configural processing in fingerprint experts , 2005, Vision Research.
[26] Arthur M. Jacobs,et al. Letter legibility and visual word recognition , 1998, Memory & cognition.
[27] T A Polk,et al. Brain localization for arbitrary stimulus categories: a simple account based on Hebbian learning. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[28] C. Price,et al. Three Distinct Ventral Occipitotemporal Regions for Reading and Object Naming , 1999, NeuroImage.
[29] David B. Pisoni,et al. Long-term memory in speech perception: Some new findings on talker variability, speaking rate and perceptual learning , 1993, Speech Commun..
[30] H. D. Gage,et al. Cortical Activity Related to Accuracy of Letter Recognition , 2000, NeuroImage.
[31] S. Petersen,et al. Activation of extrastriate and frontal cortical areas by visual words and word-like stimuli. , 1990, Science.
[32] Noam Chomsky,et al. वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .
[33] J. Rouder,et al. Absolute Identification with Simple and Complex Stimuli , 2001, Psychological science.
[34] D. Bub,et al. Priming and response selection processes in letter classification and identification tasks. , 1995 .
[35] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Human Body , 2001 .
[36] R. Salmelin,et al. Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.
[37] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .
[38] Cindy M. Bukach,et al. Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[39] E. T. Possing,et al. Neural Correlates of Lexical Access during Visual Word Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[40] Linda B. Smith,et al. Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism? , 1996, Cognition.
[41] Marilyn Jager Adams,et al. Models of word recognition , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.
[42] B. Rapp. The nature of sublexical orthographic organization: The bigram trough hypothesis examined , 1992 .
[43] M. Tarr,et al. Orientation Priming of Novel Shapes in the Context of Viewpoint-Dependent Recognition , 1997, Perception.
[44] P. Downing,et al. Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[45] M Coltheart,et al. DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. , 2001, Psychological review.
[46] A. Healy,et al. Letter detection: A window to unitization and other cognitive processes in reading text , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[47] E. Mark Gold,et al. Language Identification in the Limit , 1967, Inf. Control..
[48] Isabel Gauthier,et al. An early electrophysiological response associated with expertise in letter perception , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[49] J M Loomis,et al. Analysis of tactile and visual confusion matrices , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.
[50] V. Sloutsky,et al. Attentional learning and flexible induction: how mundane mechanisms give rise to smart behaviors. , 2008, Child development.
[51] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[52] T. Carlson,et al. Patterns of Activity in the Categorical Representations of Objects , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[53] I. Gauthier,et al. A defense of the subordinate-level expertise account for the N170 component , 2002, Cognition.
[54] R. Malach,et al. The topography of high-order human object areas , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[55] C. J. Price,et al. Cortical localisation of the visual and auditory word form areas: A reconsideration of the evidence , 2003, Brain and Language.
[56] J. Tanaka,et al. A Neural Basis for Expert Object Recognition , 2001, Psychological science.
[57] David J. Field,et al. Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images , 1996, Nature.
[58] Tomaso Poggio,et al. Generalization in vision and motor control , 2004, Nature.
[59] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. How to Make the Word-Length Effect Disappear in Letter-by-Letter Dyslexia , 2005, Psychological science.
[60] Sing-Hang Cheung,et al. The case for the visual span as a sensory bottleneck in reading. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[61] S. Thorpe,et al. How parallel is visual processing in the ventral pathway? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[62] T R Jordan,et al. The Illusory-Letters Phenomenon: An Illustration of Graphemic Restoration in Visual Word Recognition , 1999, Perception.
[63] M. Posner. Chronometric explorations of mind , 1978 .
[64] Robert J. Crutcher,et al. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. , 1993 .
[65] T. Allison,et al. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Perception in Humans , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[66] Bidyut Baran Chaudhuri,et al. Extraction of type style-based meta-information from imaged documents , 2001, International Journal on Document Analysis and Recognition.
[67] Joseph T Devlin,et al. The myth of the visual word form area , 2003, NeuroImage.
[68] T. Sanocki. Time course of object identification: evidence for a global-to-local contingency. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[69] Thomas Sanocki,et al. Font Regularity Constraints on the Process of Letter Recognition , 1988 .
[70] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.
[71] M. Tarr,et al. Visual expertise with nonface objects leads to competition with the early perceptual processing of faces in the human occipitotemporal cortex. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[72] N. Kanwisher,et al. Face perception: domain specific, not process specific. , 2004, Neuron.
[73] A. V. D. Heijden,et al. Anempirical interletter confusionmatrix for continuous-line capitals , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[74] J. Tanaka,et al. Object categories and expertise: Is the basic level in the eye of the beholder? , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.
[75] K. James. Sensori-motor experience leads to changes in visual processing in the developing brain. , 2010, Developmental science.
[76] K. James,et al. The role of sensorimotor learning in the perception of letter-like forms: Tracking the causes of neural specialization for letters , 2009, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[77] Tracey D. Berger,et al. Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[78] N. Kanwisher,et al. Can generic expertise explain special processing for faces? , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[79] M. Sigman,et al. Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.7 July 2005 The neural code for written words: a proposal , 2022 .
[80] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.
[81] M. Farah,et al. Neural Specialization for Letter Recognition , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[82] D. Perrett,et al. Evidence accumulation in cell populations responsive to faces: an account of generalisation of recognition without mental transformations , 1998, Cognition.
[83] D. Wilkin,et al. Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.
[84] N. Kanwisher,et al. Domain specificity in visual cortex. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[85] I. Biederman,et al. Scene perception: Detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[86] C. J. Álvarez,et al. Syllable Frequency and Visual Word Recognition in Spanish , 1993 .
[87] R. Treiman,et al. Are there onset- and rime-like units in printed words? , 1987 .
[88] Jean-Claude Gilhodes,et al. Learning through Hand- or Typewriting Influences Visual Recognition of New Graphic Shapes: Behavioral and Functional Imaging Evidence , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[89] James L. McClelland,et al. An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .
[90] I. Gauthier,et al. An analysis of letter expertise in a levels-of-categorization framework , 2007 .
[91] G. M. Reicher. Perceptual recognition as a function of meaninfulness of stimulus material. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[92] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area , 2004, NeuroImage.
[93] Talma Hendler,et al. Center–periphery organization of human object areas , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[94] J. Townsend. Theoretical analysis of an alphabetic confusion matrix , 1971 .
[95] D. Besner,et al. Basic processes in reading: On the development of cross-case letter matching without reference to phonology , 1987 .
[96] T. Allison,et al. Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe , 1994, Nature.
[97] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. A module for the visual representation of faces , 1996, NeuroImage.
[98] S. Lupker. On the nature of perceptual information during letter perception , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[99] David L. Sheinberg,et al. Visual object recognition. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.
[100] Elinor McKone,et al. Orientation invariance in naming rotated objects: Individual differences and repetition priming , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[101] R. Salmelin,et al. Dynamics of letter string perception in the human occipitotemporal cortex. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[102] J. Henderson,et al. Does consistent scene context facilitate object perception? , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[103] R. Treiman,et al. How to see a reading unit , 1986 .
[104] T. Kuhn,et al. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. , 1964 .
[105] M. Arguin,et al. Jules Dejerine and His Interpretation of Pure Alexia , 1993, Brain and Language.
[106] Douglas R. Hofstadter,et al. Letter Spirit: esthetic perception and creative play in the rich microcosm of the Roman alphabet , 1995 .
[107] Ying Liu,et al. The lexical constituency model: some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. , 2005, Psychological review.
[108] Alice J. O'Toole,et al. Partially Distributed Representations of Objects and Faces in Ventral Temporal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[109] T A Polk,et al. The neural development and organization of letter recognition: evidence from functional neuroimaging, computational modeling, and behavioral studies. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[110] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Task Decomposition Through Competition in a Modular Connectionist Architecture: The What and Where Vision Tasks , 1990, Cogn. Sci..
[111] S. Palmer. Hierarchical structure in perceptual representation , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.
[112] Rafael Malach,et al. Functional analysis of the periphery effect in human building related areas , 2004, Human brain mapping.
[113] Rebecca F. Schwarzlose,et al. Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[114] N. Logothetis,et al. Psychophysical and physiological evidence for viewer-centered object representations in the primate. , 1995, Cerebral cortex.
[115] M. Tarr,et al. The Fusiform Face Area is Part of a Network that Processes Faces at the Individual Level , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[116] David D. Cox,et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) “brain reading”: detecting and classifying distributed patterns of fMRI activity in human visual cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.
[117] Denis G. Pelli,et al. The remarkable inefficiency of word recognition , 2003, Nature.
[118] T. Allison,et al. Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[119] T Poggio,et al. Regularization Algorithms for Learning That Are Equivalent to Multilayer Networks , 1990, Science.
[120] G Jobard,et al. Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies , 2003, NeuroImage.
[121] T. Sanocki. Perception : Font-Specific , Schematic Tuning , 2011 .
[122] I. Gauthier,et al. Perceptual interference supports a non-modular account of face processing , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[123] Rafael Malach,et al. Large-Scale Mirror-Symmetry Organization of Human Occipito-Temporal Object Areas , 2003, Neuron.
[124] G. Humphreys,et al. Disruption to word or letter processing? The origins of case-mixing effects. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[125] N. Kanwisher,et al. The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[126] S Lehéricy,et al. The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[127] Isabel Gauthier,et al. What constrains the organization of the ventral temporal cortex? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[128] S. Dehaene,et al. Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[129] Olli Viikki,et al. An integrated study of speaker normalisation and HMM adaptation for noise robust speaker-independent speech recognition , 2002, Speech Commun..
[130] P. Bartolomeo,et al. Visually- and motor-based knowledge of letters: evidence from a pure alexic patient , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[131] John D. Bransford,et al. New approaches to instruction: because wisdom can't be told , 1989 .
[132] Sven Joubert,et al. Neural correlates of lexical and sublexical processes in reading , 2004, Brain and Language.
[133] M. Tarr,et al. Unraveling mechanisms for expert object recognition: bridging brain activity and behavior. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[134] G. Cottrell,et al. Seeing Blobs as Faces or Letters: Modeling Effects on Discrimination , 2004 .
[135] S. Dehaene,et al. Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[136] K. Grill-Spector,et al. High-resolution imaging reveals highly selective nonface clusters in the fusiform face area , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[137] Guinevere F. Eden,et al. Attention to single letters activates left extrastriate cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.
[138] J. Bowers,et al. Orthographic, phonological, and articulatory contributions to masked letter and word priming. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[139] Shinsuke Shimojo,et al. Character complexity and redundancy in writing systems over human history , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[140] Michael J. Cosky,et al. The role of letter recognition in word recognition , 1976, Memory & cognition.
[141] T. Poggio,et al. A network that learns to recognize three-dimensional objects , 1990, Nature.
[142] R Treiman. To what extent do orthographic units in print mirror phonological units in speech? , 1994, Journal of psycholinguistic research.
[143] N Weisstein,et al. Visual Detection of Line Segments: An Object-Superiority Effect , 1974, Science.
[144] C. Frith,et al. Inattentional blindness versus inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. , 1999, Science.
[145] H H Bülthoff,et al. Psychophysical support for a two-dimensional view interpolation theory of object recognition. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[146] Isabel Gauthier,et al. Font Tuning Associated with Expertise in Letter Perception , 2006, Perception.
[147] Y. Miyashita,et al. Neural organization for the long-term memory of paired associates , 1991, Nature.
[148] T. Poggio,et al. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[149] R. K. Simpson. Nature Neuroscience , 2022 .
[150] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Computational Consequences of a Bias toward Short Connections , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[151] Talma Hendler,et al. Eccentricity Bias as an Organizing Principle for Human High-Order Object Areas , 2002, Neuron.
[152] R S Berndt,et al. A new model of letter string encoding: simulating right neglect dyslexia. , 1999, Progress in brain research.
[153] M I Posner,et al. Chronometric analysis of classification. , 1967, Psychological review.
[154] T. Allison,et al. Differential Sensitivity of Human Visual Cortex to Faces, Letterstrings, and Textures: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[155] R. Kimchi,et al. Influence of Past Experience on Perceptual Grouping , 2002, Psychological science.
[156] J. L. Mcclelland. Preliminary letter identification in the perception of words and nonwords. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[157] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Distinct unimodal and multimodal regions for word processing in the left temporal cortex , 2004, NeuroImage.
[158] K. James,et al. Letter processing automatically recruits a sensory–motor brain network , 2006, Neuropsychologia.
[159] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. The Effects of Case Mixing on Word Recognition: Evidence from a PET Study , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[160] Yaoda Xu. Revisiting the role of the fusiform face area in visual expertise. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[161] D. Mewhort,et al. Mechanisms of word identification. , 1977 .
[162] S. Dehaene,et al. The visual word form area: a prelexical representation of visual words in the fusiform gyrus , 2002, Neuroreport.
[163] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[164] S. Goldinger. Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. , 1998, Psychological review.
[165] M. Posner,et al. Retention of visual and name codes of single letters. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[166] P. Jolicoeur,et al. Orientation congruency effects on the identification of disoriented shapes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[167] N. F. Johnson,et al. A Cohort Model of Visual Word Recognition , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.
[168] T Sanocki. Looking for a Structural Network: Effects of Changing Size and Style on Letter Recognition , 1991, Perception.
[169] T. Sanocki. Effects of font- and letter-specific experience on the perceptual processing of letters , 1992 .
[170] Jean-Luc Velay,et al. Visual presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in writing , 2003, NeuroImage.
[171] W. Schneider,et al. Cross‐cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation , 2005, Human brain mapping.
[172] Arnaud Rey,et al. Graphemes are perceptual reading units , 2000, Cognition.
[173] H. Bouma. Visual recognition of isolated lower-case letters. , 1971, Vision research.
[174] M. Tagamets,et al. A Parametric Approach to Orthographic Processing in the Brain: An fMRI Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[175] G C Gilmore,et al. Multidimensional letter similarity derived from recognition errors , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[176] R W Schvaneveldt,et al. An activation--verification model for letter and word recognition: the word-superiority effect. , 1982, Psychological review.
[177] Stephen J. Boies,et al. Components of attention. , 1971 .
[178] Wayne D. Gray,et al. Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.
[179] M. Tarr,et al. FFA: a flexible fusiform area for subordinate-level visual processing automatized by expertise , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[180] Alice F. Healy,et al. Detection errors onthe andand: Evidence for reading units larger than the word , 1977, Memory & cognition.
[181] James L. McClelland,et al. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.