Early-holistic processing or dimensional similarity?

In this commentary, we consider current evidence for the theory that multidimensional stimuli are initially processed as unanalyzed wholes. The theory appears unable to explain observers' behavior in various speeded and unspeeded tasks: (a) Perceivers can differentiate tiny changes made between confusable dimensions; (b) the fits of distance metrics to rating data appear to be largely under the control of optional processes; and (c) direct predictions from early-holistic models, as instantiated in the Euclidean hypothesis, are disconfirmed in tests using the rotation paradigm. In contrast, evidence has mounted to support the view that perception relies on a set of primary dimensions, processed within context-bound constraints. D. G. Kemler Nelson (1993) has been critical of our approach; we respond to each of her objections. Kemler Nelson (1993) summarizes the traditional view of how perceptual dimensions interact, what we call the earlyholistic theory. According to this theory, stimuli formed from interacting dimensions (e.g., colors varying in saturation, brightness, and hue) are first perceived holistically, as if they are unanalyzed points in a dimensionless space and then, with cognitive effort or processing time, are analyzed into their dimensional components. Early-holistic theory has been a fertile approach to the study of multidimensional perception. Over the past 20 years, Kemler Nelson and her colleagues have assembled a plethora of data and interpreted all of it through this system. In this short article, we are obviously unable to address this large body of data in detail. Nonetheless, by considering some of the main features of Kemler Nelson's (1993) summary we hope to raise substantive doubts about the continued usefulness of the theory. Early-holistic models are predicated on the logic of converging operations, which considers no single outcome to be decisive in accepting or rejecting a theory (Garner, 1974; Garner, Hake, & Eriksen, 1956). It should come as no surprise, therefore, that it is possible to question individual findings and interpretations even if the entire theoretical approach were sound. Nonetheless, we believe that the doubts we raise are so fundamental that they enervate the actual structure of the theory.

[1]  R. Melara,et al.  Dimensional interaction between color and pitch. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  R. Melara,et al.  Selective attention to Stroop dimensions: Effects of baseline discriminability, response mode, and practice , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[3]  R. Melara,et al.  Contextual influences on interactive processing: Effects of discriminability, quantity, and uncertainty , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  A response time theory of separability and integrality in speeded classification , 1994 .

[5]  W. Todd Maddox,et al.  Perceptual and decisional separability. , 1992 .

[6]  D. G. Kemler Classification in young and retarded children: the primacy of overall similarity relations. , 1982, Child development.

[7]  S. Carey,et al.  Anatomy of a stimulus domain: The relation between multidimensional and unidimensional scaling of noise bands , 1975 .

[8]  L. Marks,et al.  Primacy of dimensions in color perception. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Daniel Algom,et al.  Psychophysical approaches to cognition. , 1992 .

[10]  Miguelina Guirao,et al.  Measurement of Auditory Density , 1964 .

[11]  D. Hubel,et al.  Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: anatomy, physiology, and perception. , 1988, Science.

[12]  N. Perrin,et al.  Varieties of perceptual independence. , 1986, Psychological review.

[13]  C. F. Foard,et al.  Holistic and analytic modes of processing: the multiple determinants of perceptual analysis. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[14]  Robert D. Sorkin,et al.  Extension of the Theory of Signal Detectability to Matching Procedures in Psychoacoustics , 1962 .

[15]  L E Marks,et al.  Perceptual primacy of dimensions: support for a model of dimensional interaction. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  D. K. Nelson,et al.  Processing integral dimensions: the whole view. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  S. Zeki The representation of colours in the cerebral cortex , 1980, Nature.

[18]  Robert D. Melara,et al.  The concept of perceptual similarity : from psychophysics to cognitive psychology , 1992 .

[19]  S. Imai,et al.  The free classification of analyzable and unanalyzable stimuli , 1972 .

[20]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  The quantification of tonal volume. , 1962, The American journal of psychology.

[21]  W. Kintsch,et al.  Memory and cognition , 1977 .

[22]  R. Melara,et al.  Similarity relations among synesthetic stimuli and their attributes. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  L. Marks,et al.  Optional processes in similarity judgments , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[25]  Robert D. Melara,et al.  Interaction between synesthetically corresponding dimensions. , 1987 .

[26]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Levels of experienced dimensionality in children and adults , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[27]  N. Graham Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .

[28]  W. R. Garner,et al.  Operationism and the concept of perception. , 1956, Psychological review.

[29]  D. Day,et al.  Primacy of dimensions in vibrotactile perception: An evaluation of early holistic models , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[30]  T Carvellas,et al.  Direct estimation of multidimensional tonal dissimilarity. , 1972, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  G. Lockhead Processing dimensional stimuli: a note. , 1972, Psychological review.

[32]  J W Grau,et al.  The distinction between integral and separable dimensions: evidence for the integrality of pitch and loudness. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[33]  S. Zeki Functional specialisation in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1978, Nature.

[34]  W. R. Garner The Processing of Information and Structure , 1974 .

[35]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Overall similarity in adults' classification: The child in all of us. , 1984 .

[36]  L. Marks,et al.  Hard andsoft interacting dimensions: Differential effects of dual context on classification , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[37]  W. R. Garner,et al.  Integrality of stimulus dimensions in various types of information processing , 1970 .

[38]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .