What's in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self.

This article explores the links between implicit self-esteem and the automatic self (D. L. Paulhus, 1993). Across 4 studies, name letter evaluations were positively biased, confirming that implicit self-esteem is generally positive (A. G. Greenwald & M. R. Banaji, 1995). Study 1 found that this name letter bias was stable over a 4-week period. Study 2 found that positive bias for name letters and positive bias for birth date numbers were correlated and that both biases became inhibited when participants were induced to respond in a deliberative manner. Studies 3-4 found that implicit self-evaluations corresponded with self-reported self-evaluations, but only when participants were evaluating themselves very quickly (Study 3) or under cognitive load (Study 4). Together, these findings support the notion that implicit self-esteem phenomena are driven by self-evaluations that are activated automatically and without conscious self-reflection.

[1]  V. Hoorens Nuttin's affective selfparticles hypothesis and the name letter effect: A review. , 1990 .

[2]  D. A. Gold,et al.  Mental representations of the self, significant others, and nonsignificant others: structure and processing of private and public aspects. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  Sander L. Koole,et al.  Overcoming the planning fallacy through willpower : effects of implementation intentions on actual and predicted task-completion times , 2000 .

[4]  A. Tesser,et al.  On the substitutability of self-protective mechanisms , 1996 .

[5]  J. M. Nuttin,et al.  Overvaluation of own attributes: Mere ownership or subjective frequency? , 1993 .

[6]  Mark W. Baldwin,et al.  Priming relationship schemas: My advisor and the pope are watching me from the back of my mind , 1990 .

[7]  The initial letter effect : ego-attachment or mere exposure? / , 1986 .

[8]  D T Gilbert,et al.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology the Fleeting Gleam of Praise Cognitive Processes Underlying Behavioral Reactions to Self-relevant Feedback , 2022 .

[9]  A. Fernald Approval and disapproval: infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. , 1993, Child development.

[10]  J. Kruger Lake Wobegon be gone! The "below-average effect" and the egocentric nature of comparative ability judgments. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[11]  John A. Johnson,et al.  Handbook of personality psychology. , 1997 .

[12]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[13]  C. Sedikides Assessment, enhancement, and verification determinants of the self-evaluation process. , 1993 .

[14]  John Bowlby,et al.  Separation: Anxiety and Anger , 1973 .

[15]  Duane T. Wegener,et al.  The Flexible Correction Model: The Role of Naive Theories of Bias in Bias Correction , 1997 .

[16]  F. Strack,et al.  Awareness of influence as a precondition for implementing correctional goals. , 1996 .

[17]  J G Hixon,et al.  When does introspection bear fruit? Self-reflection, self-insight, and interpersonal choices. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[18]  Eliot R. Smith,et al.  Development of automatism of social judgments. , 1986 .

[19]  J. M. Nuttin Affective consequences of mere ownership: The name letter effect in twelve European languages , 1987 .

[20]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  The impact of accuracy motivation on interpretation, comparison, and correction processes: accuracy x knowledge accessibility effects. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[21]  Frank Bernhard Brokken,et al.  The language of personality , 1978 .

[22]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Relationship between attitudes and evaluative space: A critical review, with emphasis on the separability of positive and negative substrates. , 1994 .

[23]  Jonathon D. Brown Self-esteem and self-evaluation: Feeling is believing. , 1993 .

[24]  D. Stipek,et al.  Declining Perceptions of Competence: A Consequence of Changes in the Child or in the Educational Environment?. , 1988 .

[25]  S. Fiske,et al.  The Handbook of Social Psychology , 1935 .

[26]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Psychological perspectives on the self , 1982 .

[27]  A. J. Smith,et al.  The relationship between attitudes and beliefs. , 1973, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  P Haggard,et al.  The psychology of action. , 2001, British journal of psychology.

[29]  G. W. Bradley Self-serving biases in the attribution process: A reexamination of the fact or fiction question. , 1978 .

[30]  D. Watson,et al.  Toward a consensual structure of mood. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.

[31]  J. Bowlby Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. , 1969, The American journal of orthopsychiatry.

[32]  J. Greenberg,et al.  A dual-process model of defense against conscious and unconscious death-related thoughts: an extension of terror management theory. , 1999, Psychological review.

[33]  W. Swann,et al.  From self-conceptions to self-worth: on the sources and structure of global self-esteem. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[34]  B. Black,et al.  The relationship between attitudes , 1998 .

[35]  D T Gilbert,et al.  When comparisons arise. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[36]  B. Pelham,et al.  Why Susie sells seashells by the seashore: implicit egotism and major life decisions. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[37]  James W. Pennebaker,et al.  The Handbook of Mental Control , 1992 .

[38]  Vera Hoorens,et al.  Self‐Favoring Biases, Self‐Presentation, and the Self‐Other Asymmetry in Social Comparison , 1995 .

[39]  L. Wheeler,et al.  Review of personality and social psychology , 1980 .

[40]  M. Leary,et al.  INTERPERSONAL FUNCTIONS OF THE SELF-ESTEEM MOTIVE THE SELF-ESTEEM SYSTEM AS A SOCIOMETER , 1995 .

[41]  J. Bargh,et al.  Automaticity in action: The unconscious as repository of chronic goals and motives. , 1996 .

[42]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Effects of analyzing reasons on attitude–behavior consistency. , 1984 .

[43]  K. J. Cairns,et al.  Repression and self-presentation: when audiences interfere with self-deceptive strategies. , 1992, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[44]  Mark Muraven,et al.  When modesty prevails: Differential favorability of self-presentation to friends and strangers. , 1995 .

[45]  S. Epstein,et al.  The Missing Link in the Paradox of the Linda Conjunction Problem: Beyond Knowing and Thinking of the Conjunction Rule, the Intrinsic Appeal of Heuristic Processing , 1999 .

[46]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  Data analysis in social psychology. , 1998 .

[47]  C. Dweck,et al.  The development of trait explanations and self-evaluations in the academic and social domains. , 1986, Child development.

[48]  J. Bargh The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. , 1994 .

[49]  Delroy L. Paulhus,et al.  Psychological Defense: Contemporary Theory and Research , 1997 .

[50]  David M. Sanbonmatsu,et al.  On the automatic activation of attitudes. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[51]  E. S. Knowles,et al.  Why people say "yes": A dual-process theory of acquiescence. , 1999 .

[52]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[53]  M. Kernis Efficacy, Agency, and Self-Esteem , 1995 .

[54]  Michael Lewis,et al.  Social cognition and the acquisition of self , 1979 .

[55]  D. Wentura,et al.  About the impact of automaticity in the minimal group paradigm: evidence from affective priming tasks , 1999 .

[56]  A. Dijksterhuis,et al.  Memory for stereotype‐consistent and stereotype‐inconsistent information as a function of processing pace , 1995 .

[57]  R. Zajonc,et al.  Affect, cognition, and awareness: affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[58]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.

[59]  S. Epstein,et al.  Is the self motivated to do more than enhance and/or verify itself? , 1995 .

[60]  J. M. Nuttin Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: The name letter effect , 1985 .

[61]  B. Pelham,et al.  Two Roads to Positive Regard: Implicit and Explicit Self-Evaluation and Culture , 1999 .

[62]  J. D. Brown,et al.  The thrill of victory, the complexity of defeat: self-esteem and people's emotional reactions to success and failure. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[63]  Tom Pyszczynski,et al.  A Terror Management Theory of Social Behavior: The Psychological Functions of Self-Esteem and Cultural Worldviews , 1991 .

[64]  S. Kitayama,et al.  Implicit Self-Esteem in Japan: Name Letters and Birthday Numbers , 1997 .

[65]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[66]  E. Erikson Identity, youth, and crisis , 1968 .

[67]  T. Chartrand,et al.  THE UNBEARABLE AUTOMATICITY OF BEING , 1999 .

[68]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Parts Outweigh the Whole (Word) in Unconscious Analysis of Meaning , 2000, Psychological science.

[69]  William B. Swann,et al.  The Search for Beauty and Truth: A Framework for Understanding Reactions to Evaluations , 1995 .

[70]  Salience and the Cognitive Mediation of Attribution , 1979 .

[71]  B. Pelham,et al.  On confidence and consequence: the certainty and importance of self-knowledge. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[72]  S. Chaiken,et al.  The Automatic Evaluation Effect: Unconditional Automatic Attitude Activation with a Pronunciation Task , 1996 .

[73]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  A model of dual attitudes. , 2000, Psychological review.

[74]  M. Selst,et al.  ATTENTIONAL LOAD INCREASES THE POSITIVITY OF SELF-PRESENTATION , 1989 .

[75]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Motivated closing of the mind: "seizing" and "freezing". , 1996, Psychological review.

[76]  James K. Beggan On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. , 1992 .

[77]  Stephen Hughes Attachment and Loss. Vol. I , 1969, Mental Health.

[78]  Y. Eshel,et al.  Development of Academic Self-Concept of Lower-Class and Middle-Class Primary School Children. , 1981 .

[79]  D. Paulhus,et al.  Desirable Responding Triggered by Affect: Automatic Egotism? , 1987 .

[80]  V. Hoorens,et al.  The name letter effect: Attachment to self or primacy of own name writing? , 1988 .

[81]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Introspection, Attitude Change, and Attitude-Behavior Consistency: the Disruptive Effects of Explaining Why we Feel the Way we Do , 1989 .

[82]  Anthony G. Greenwald,et al.  Implicit self-esteem. , 1999 .

[83]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  Terror management and cognitive-experiential self-theory: evidence that terror management occurs in the experiential system. , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[84]  J. M. Nuttin,et al.  Mastery pleasure versus mere ownership: A quasi-experimental cross-cultural and cross-alphabetical test of the name letter effect , 1990 .

[85]  M. Ross Relation of Implicit Theories to the Construction of Personal Histories , 1989 .

[86]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Handbook of Social Cognition , 1993 .

[87]  H. Markus,et al.  Possible Selves , 2001 .

[88]  Mario Mikulincer,et al.  Attachment style and the mental representation of the self. , 1995 .

[89]  Sander L. Koole,et al.  The Cessation of Rumination Through Self-Affirmation , 1999 .

[90]  C. D. De Dreu,et al.  Unfixing the fixed pie: a motivated information-processing approach to integrative negotiation. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[91]  M. Strube,et al.  Self-Evaluation: To Thine Own Self Be Good, To Thine Own Self Be Sure, To Thine Own Self Be True, an , 1997 .

[92]  B. Woike,et al.  Most-memorable experiences: evidence for a link between implicit and explicit motives and social cognitive processes in everyday life , 1995 .

[93]  Constantine Sedikides,et al.  The self-serving bias in relational context. , 1998 .

[94]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[95]  S. Epstein Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. , 1994, The American psychologist.