Flexible emotion-based decision-making behavior varies in current and former smokers.

INTRODUCTION Suboptimal decision-making is a feature in the initiation and maintenance of substance use, often manifested in choosing for short-term benefits rather than long-term gain, and the failure to display cognitive flexibility, respectively. Studies of nicotine users typically focus on characterizing those who are already addicted; less is known about decision-making in former smokers. METHODS Non- (n=21), former daily- (n=23) and current daily smokers (n=24), completed the contingency-shifting variant Iowa Gambling Task (csIGT), in which the reward and punishment contingencies of the decks are systematically varied after 100 trials of the 'standard' IGT. Scores on the standard blocks of the csIGT provided an index of emotion-based decision-making, while the contingency-shifting blocks assessed flexible decision-making. Subjective ratings were also recorded at 20-trial intervals. RESULTS Both current and former smokers showed significantly impaired performance relative to non-smokers when making decisions during the standard blocks of the csIGT. Both former and non-smokers' awareness of the reward/punishment contingencies was significantly higher than those of current smokers at the end of the standard IGT. Both former and non-smokers had significantly better performance on the contingency shifting blocks, relative to current smokers. CONCLUSIONS The findings indicate that both current and former smokers display a suboptimal pattern of decision-making than non-smokers during the standard IGT. However, with respect to the ability to change behavior following reversed contingencies, former smokers are more similar to non-smokers than to current smokers. Former smokers were also more aware of the contingencies than current smokers.

[1]  S. Molinaro,et al.  Concordance and consistency of answers to the self‐delivered ESPAD questionnaire on use of psychoactive substances , 2012, International journal of methods in psychiatric research.

[2]  Suzanne H. Mitchell,et al.  Differences in delay discounting between smokers and nonsmokers remain when both rewards are delayed , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[3]  J. Flory,et al.  Delay discounting and smoking: association with the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence but not cigarettes smoked per day. , 2008, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[4]  Zsófia Osváth,et al.  DOI: 10 , 2011 .

[5]  Melissa T. Buelow,et al.  Risky Decision Making in Smoking and Nonsmoking College Students: Examination of Iowa Gambling Task Performance by Deck Type Selections , 2014, Applied neuropsychology. Child.

[6]  A. Cooper,et al.  Impaired flexible decision-making in Major Depressive Disorder. , 2010, Journal of affective disorders.

[7]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[8]  J. Rusted,et al.  Degree of dependence influences the effect of smoking on cognitive flexibility , 2011, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[9]  Sohee Park,et al.  A novel set-shifting modification of the iowa gambling task: flexible emotion-based learning in schizophrenia. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[10]  A. Cooper,et al.  Effects of decision-phase time constraints on emotion-based learning in the Iowa Gambling Task , 2007, Brain and Cognition.

[11]  Karen I Bolla,et al.  The differential relationship between cocaine use and marijuana use on decision-making performance over repeat testing with the Iowa Gambling Task. , 2007, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[12]  J. Richards,et al.  Delay discounting and probability discounting as related to cigarette smoking status in adults , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[13]  W. Bickel,et al.  Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. , 1998, Addiction.

[14]  Steven W Anderson,et al.  Decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in alcohol and stimulant abusers , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  S. Shoptaw,et al.  Impaired performance in a test of decision-making by opiate-dependent tobacco smokers. , 2004, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[16]  J. Jolles,et al.  Cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in relation to cognitive performance in middle age. , 2002, American journal of epidemiology.

[17]  Vandana Sundaram,et al.  The American Journal of Public Health. , 1945, American journal of public health and the nation's health.

[18]  M. Rietschel,et al.  Neuropsychosocial profiles of current and future adolescent alcohol misusers , 2014, Nature.

[19]  A. Damasio,et al.  Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  G. Heyman,et al.  Decision biases and persistent illicit drug use: an experimental study of distributed choice and addiction. , 2002, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[21]  J. Weatherly,et al.  Delay and probability discounting of multiple commodities in smokers and never-smokers using multiple-choice tasks , 2013, Behavioural pharmacology.

[22]  O. Turnbull,et al.  Subjective Awareness on the Iowa Gambling Task: The Key Role of Emotional Experience in Schizophrenia , 2005, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[23]  O. Turnbull,et al.  Artificial time constraints on the Iowa Gambling Task: The effects on behavioural performance and subjective experience , 2005, Brain and Cognition.

[24]  T. Bjarnason,et al.  The 2011 ESPAD report: substance use among students in 36 European countries. , 2012 .

[25]  S. Mitchell,et al.  Effects of short-term nicotine deprivation on decision-making: delay, uncertainty and effort discounting. , 2004, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[26]  Donna J. Calu,et al.  Withdrawal from cocaine self-administration produces long-lasting deficits in orbitofrontal-dependent reversal learning in rats. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[27]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: what participants really know in the Iowa gambling task. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[28]  Will M Aklin,et al.  The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. , 2003, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[29]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[30]  A. Cooper,et al.  Impairment in flexible emotion-based learning in hallucination- and delusion-prone individuals , 2009, Psychiatry Research.

[31]  O. García-Rodríguez,et al.  Decreased delay discounting in former cigarette smokers at one year after treatment. , 2014, Addictive behaviors.

[32]  A. Bechara,et al.  Decision-making and addiction (part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  Hanna Damasio,et al.  Decision-making and addiction (part I): impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  Melissa T. Buelow,et al.  Construct Validity of the Iowa Gambling Task , 2009, Neuropsychology Review.

[35]  M. Pérez-García,et al.  Profile of executive deficits in cocaine and heroin polysubstance users: common and differential effects on separate executive components , 2007, Psychopharmacology.

[36]  H. Damasio,et al.  Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[37]  A. Bechara Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  M. Farah,et al.  Different underlying impairments in decision-making following ventromedial and dorsolateral frontal lobe damage in humans. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  E. Claus,et al.  Age differences in affective decision making as indexed by performance on the Iowa Gambling Task. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[40]  Marc A Schuckit,et al.  Behavioral and Functional Neuroimaging Evidence for Prefrontal Dysfunction in Methamphetamine-Dependent Subjects , 2002, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[41]  Darla E Kendzor,et al.  The relative impact of nicotine dependence, other substance dependence, and gender on Bechara Gambling Task performance. , 2008, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[42]  Jean-Lud Cadet,et al.  Frontal cortical tissue composition in abstinent cocaine abusers: a magnetic resonance imaging study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[43]  O. Turnbull,et al.  Paradoxical effects of education on the Iowa Gambling Task , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[44]  Liam Nestor,et al.  Differences in “bottom-up” and “top-down” neural activity in current and former cigarette smokers: Evidence for neural substrates which may promote nicotine abstinence through increased cognitive control , 2011, NeuroImage.

[45]  P. Courtet,et al.  Somatic markers and explicit knowledge are both involved in decision-making , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Matteo Cella,et al.  Cognitive decision modelling of emotion-based learning impairment in schizophrenia: The role of awareness , 2012, Psychiatry Research.

[47]  Michael A. Nader,et al.  The effects of cocaine: A shifting target over the course of addiction , 2007, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[48]  Jerry L. Grenard,et al.  Affective decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in 10th-grade Chinese adolescent smokers. , 2008, Nicotine & tobacco research : official journal of the Society for Research on Nicotine and Tobacco.

[49]  Fernando Zelaya,et al.  Distinct roles of prefrontal cortical subregions in the Iowa Gambling Task. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  W. Verschuren,et al.  Smoking and cognitive decline among middle-aged men and women: the Doetinchem Cohort Study. , 2008, American journal of public health.

[51]  E. Yechiam,et al.  Smokers’ Decision Making: More than Mere Risk Taking , 2013, PloS one.

[52]  A. Cooper,et al.  The contingency-shifting variant Iowa Gambling Task: An investigation with young adults , 2010, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[53]  Kris N Kirby,et al.  Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. , 2004, Addiction.

[54]  Y. Niv,et al.  The impact of orbitofrontal dysfunction on cocaine addiction , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  J E Steinmetz,et al.  Decision-making biases, antisocial personality, and early-onset alcoholism. , 2000, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[56]  A. Odum,et al.  Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers , 1999, Psychopharmacology.

[57]  Carlo Contoreggi,et al.  Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  Joseph A Maldjian,et al.  Decreased gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine patients , 2002, Biological Psychiatry.

[59]  A. Lawrence,et al.  The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[60]  G. Rauchs,et al.  Impaired decision-making and brain shrinkage in alcoholism , 2014, European Psychiatry.

[61]  Antoine Bechara,et al.  A somatic marker theory of addiction , 2009, Neuropharmacology.

[62]  P. Laurienti,et al.  Long-term heavy marijuana users make costly decisions on a gambling task. , 2004, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[63]  R. Whelan,et al.  The neurobiology of successful abstinence , 2013, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[64]  A. Damasio,et al.  Deciding Advantageously Before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy , 1997, Science.