Stereo sensitivity depends on stereo matching.

Stereoacuity thresholds, measured with bar targets, rise as the absolute disparity of the bars is increased. One explanation for this rise is that, as the bars are moved away from the fixation plane, the stereo system uses coarser mechanisms to encode the bars' disparity; coarse mechanisms are insensitive to small changes in target disparity, resulting in higher thresholds. To test this explanation, we measured stereoacuity with a 6 degrees wide 3 cpd grating presented in a rectangular envelope. We varied the disparity of the grating and its edges (envelope) parametrically from 0 to 20 arcmin (i.e., through one full period). To force observers to make judgments based on carrier disparity, we then varied the interocular phase incrementally from trial-to-trial while keeping edge disparity fixed for a given block of trials. The pedestal phase disparity of the grating necessarily cycles through 360 degrees, back to zero disparity, as the edge disparity increases monotonically from 0 to 20 arcmin. Unlike mechanisms that respond to bars, the mechanism that responds to the interocular phase disparity of the grating should have the same sensitivity at 20 arcmin disparity (360 degrees of phase) as it has at zero disparity. So, if stereoacuity were determined by the most sensitive mechanism, thresholds should oscillate with the pedestal phase disparity. However, these gratings are perceived in depth at the disparity of their edges. If stereoacuity were instead determined by the stereo matching operations that generate perceived depth, thresholds should rise monotonically with increasing edge disparity. We found that the rise in grating thresholds with increasing edge disparity was monotonic and virtually identical to the rise in thresholds observed for bars. Stereoacuity is contingent on stereo matching.

[1]  C. Blakemore The range and scope of binocular depth discrimination in man , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  K. N. Ogle Precision and validity of stereoscopic depth perception from double images. , 1953, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[3]  Laurie M. Wilcox,et al.  Linear and non-linear filtering in stereopsis , 1994, Vision Research.

[4]  H S Smallman,et al.  Spatial Scale Interactions in Stereo Sensitivity and the Neural Representation of Binocular Disparity , 1997, Perception.

[5]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Responses of spatial mechanisms can explain hyperacuity , 1986, Vision Research.

[6]  G. Sperling,et al.  Measuring the spatial frequency selectivity of second-order texture mechanisms , 1995, Vision Research.

[7]  S. McKee,et al.  The use of an implicit standard for measuring discrimination thresholds , 2000, Vision Research.

[8]  B. Cumming,et al.  Testing quantitative models of binocular disparity selectivity in primary visual cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  C. Schor,et al.  Disparity range for local stereopsis as a function of luminance spatial frequency , 1983, Vision Research.

[10]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Cooperative computation of stereo disparity , 1988 .

[11]  B. Julesz Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1971 .

[12]  Bart Farell,et al.  Coarse scales, fine scales, and their interactions in stereo vision. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[13]  B. Julesz,et al.  Binocular disparity modulation sensitivity to disparities offset from the plane of fixation , 1984, Vision Research.

[14]  D Marr,et al.  A computational theory of human stereo vision. , 1979, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[15]  S. McKee,et al.  The imprecision of stereopsis , 1990, Vision Research.

[16]  Mark Edwards,et al.  Spatial interactions minimize relative disparity between adjacent surfaces , 2001, Vision Research.

[17]  A. Parker,et al.  The Precision of Single Neuron Responses in Cortical Area V1 during Stereoscopic Depth Judgments , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[18]  Richard A Eagle,et al.  Weighted directional energy model of human stereo correspondence , 2000, Vision Research.

[19]  Ning Qian,et al.  Computing Stereo Disparity and Motion with Known Binocular Cell Properties , 1994, Neural Computation.

[20]  G Westheimer,et al.  Contrast and duration of exposure differentially affect vernier and stereoscopic acuity , 1990, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[21]  S. Lehky,et al.  Neural model of stereoacuity and depth interpolation based on a distributed representation of stereo disparity [published erratum appears in J Neurosci 1991 Mar;11(3):following Table of Contents] , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  Keith Langley,et al.  Stereopsis from contrast envelopes , 1999, Vision Research.

[23]  Jonathan D. Victor,et al.  Reading a population code: a multi-scale neural model for representing binocular disparity , 2003, Vision Research.

[24]  S. McKee,et al.  Bias and sensitivity of stereo judgements in the presence of a slanted reference plane , 1999, Vision Research.

[25]  M S Gray,et al.  Reliable disparity estimation through selective integration , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.

[26]  D. Badcock,et al.  Depth-increment detection function for individual spatial channels. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[27]  Richard A Eagle,et al.  Stereo correspondence in one-dimensional Gabor stimuli , 2000, Vision Research.

[28]  S. Klein,et al.  Hyperacuity thresholds of 1 sec: theoretical predictions and empirical validation. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[29]  R. Blake,et al.  How Contrast Affects Stereoacuity , 1988, Perception.

[30]  A. Parker,et al.  A specialization for relative disparity in V2 , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  David J. Fleet,et al.  Neural encoding of binocular disparity: Energy models, position shifts and phase shifts , 1996, Vision Research.

[32]  J P Frisby,et al.  PMF: A Stereo Correspondence Algorithm Using a Disparity Gradient Limit , 1985, Perception.

[33]  Laurie M Wilcox,et al.  First and second-order contributions to surface interpolation , 1999, Vision Research.

[34]  Preeti Verghese,et al.  What is the depth of a sinusoidal grating? , 2004, Journal of vision.

[35]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Disjunctive eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[36]  Andrew J. Parker,et al.  Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  A. Parker,et al.  Binocular Neurons in V1 of Awake Monkeys Are Selective for Absolute, Not Relative, Disparity , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Mark Edwards,et al.  Envelope size tuning for stereo-depth perception of small and large disparities , 2001, Vision Research.

[39]  A. Parker,et al.  Range and mechanism of encoding of horizontal disparity in macaque V1. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  S. McKee,et al.  Steroscopic acuity for moving retinal images. , 1978, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[41]  Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.  Is correspondence search in human stereo vision a coarse-to-fine process? , 1996, Biological Cybernetics.

[42]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Stereopsis and contrast , 1989, Vision Research.