Two mechanisms that determine the Barber-Pole Illusion

[1]  Hans Wallach Über visuell wahrgenommene Bewegungsrichtung , 1935 .

[2]  W. Reichardt,et al.  Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system , 1961 .

[3]  S. M. Axstis PHI MOVEMENT AS A SUBTRACTION PROCESS , 1970 .

[4]  J. van Santen,et al.  Temporal covariance model of human motion perception. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[5]  E H Adelson,et al.  Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[6]  A J Ahumada,et al.  Model of human visual-motion sensing. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[7]  J. van Santen,et al.  Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[8]  G. Sperling,et al.  Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[9]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion , 1989, Vision Research.

[10]  G Sperling,et al.  Two motion perception mechanisms revealed through distance-driven reversal of apparent motion. , 1989, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  Jonathan A. Marshall,et al.  Self-organizing neural networks for perception of visual motion , 1990, Neural Networks.

[12]  E. Adelson,et al.  Directionally selective complex cells and the computation of motion energy in cat visual cortex , 1992, Vision Research.

[13]  Maggie Shiffrar,et al.  The influence of terminators on motion integration across space , 1992, Vision Research.

[14]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Perceived direction of moving two-dimensional patterns depends on duration, contrast and eccentricity , 1992, Vision Research.

[15]  F. Kooi Local direction of edge motion causes and abolishes the barberpole illusion , 1993, Vision Research.

[16]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Different motion sensitive units are involved in recovering the direction of moving lines , 1993, Vision Research.

[17]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  Isolating the effect of one-dimensional motion signals on the perceived direction of moving two-dimensional objects , 1993, Vision Research.

[18]  George Sperling,et al.  Attention-generated apparent motion , 1995, Nature.

[19]  G. Sperling,et al.  The functional architecture of human visual motion perception , 1995, Vision Research.

[20]  L. Stone,et al.  The Barberplaid Illusion: Plaid Motion is Biased by Elongated Apertures , 1996, Vision Research.

[21]  R. Shapley,et al.  “On the Visually Perceived Direction of Motion” by Hans Wallach: 60 Years Later , 1996 .

[22]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[23]  H. Wallach On the visually perceived direction of motion ' ' by Hans Wallach : 60 years later , 1997 .

[24]  S. Wuerger,et al.  Perception of Moving Lines: Interactions Between Local Perpendicular Signals and 2D Motion Signals , 1997, Vision Research.

[25]  Ennio Mingolla,et al.  Monocular occlusion cues alter the influence of terminator motion in the barber pole phenomenon , 1998, Vision Research.

[26]  G Sperling,et al.  Second-order reversed phi , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[27]  Wilson S. Geisler,et al.  Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.

[28]  Eric Castet,et al.  The extrinsic/intrinsic classification of two-dimensional motion signals with barber-pole stimuli , 1999, Vision Research.

[29]  E. Castet,et al.  Temporal dynamics of motion integration for the initiation of tracking eye movements at ultra-short latencies , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.

[30]  Z L Lu,et al.  Three-systems theory of human visual motion perception: review and update. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[31]  J. Zanker,et al.  The Directional Tuning of the Barber-Pole Illusion , 2001, Perception.

[32]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  Allison M McKendrick,et al.  Pattern cues disambiguate perceived direction in simple moving stimuli , 2003, Vision Research.

[34]  Margaret S Livingstone,et al.  End-Stopping and the Aperture Problem Two-Dimensional Motion Signals in Macaque V1 , 2003, Neuron.

[35]  A. T. Smith,et al.  Human visual area V6 is selectively responsive to egomotion-compatible optic flow , 2009 .

[36]  Jean Lorenceau,et al.  From Moving Contours to Object Motion: Functional Networks for Visual Form/Motion Processing , 2009 .

[37]  George Sperling,et al.  A moving-barber-pole illusion. , 2014, Journal of vision.