Visual recognition memory. Neurophysiological evidence for the role of temporal white matter in man.

A novel event-related potential (ERP) elicited by a visuospatial recognition memory task was recorded in 20 patients with temporal lobe epilepsy using depth electrodes sited in the temporal lobes. The ERPs comprised two components, an N400 and a P600, and were similar in morphology to the previously reported ERP to verbal recognition memory tasks. The two ERP components in both verbal and visuospatial tasks were dependent on stimulus type and our data suggest that they do not simply represent delayed P300 ERP responses. In 17/20 patients robust, reliable bilaterally present ERPs were elicited by both verbal and visuospatial memory tasks. N400 amplitude was larger in response to novel stimuli, whereas P600 amplitude was larger to repeated stimuli. P600 amplitude was larger in the right temporal lobe to both visuospatial and verbal stimulus material. N400 and P600 latencies did not vary with task, stimulus type or side of recording. In 3/20 patients, no ERPs were elicited by either memory task. In all 3 cases, unilateral temporal white matter abnormalities were demonstrated by magnetic resonance imaging. Behavioural measures, expressed in the form of standardized accuracy scores, did not differ from those of a normal control group, and hence are unlikely to account for the abnormalities in ERPs. These results are discussed with reference to the primate visual recognition memory pathway and suggest that ERPs to recognition memory tasks are generated by an interaction between the two homologous inferotemporal recognition memory pathways.

[1]  L. R. Peterson,et al.  Short-term retention of individual verbal items. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[2]  E. John,et al.  Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty , 1965, Science.

[3]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[4]  E R John,et al.  Information Delivery and the Sensory Evoked Potential , 1967, Science.

[5]  A. Paivio,et al.  Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  B S Kopell,et al.  Effects of stimulus probability and task-relevance on event-related potentials. , 1976, Psychophysiology.

[8]  L. Poon,et al.  Average evoked potential changes as a function of processing complexity. , 1976, Psychophysiology.

[9]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[10]  E. Donchin,et al.  On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[11]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading between the lines: Event-related brain potentials during natural sentence processing , 1980, Brain and Language.

[12]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[13]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words , 1980, Biological Psychology.

[14]  J. Rohrbaugh,et al.  Endogenous potentials generated in the human hippocampal formation and amygdala by infrequent events. , 1980, Science.

[15]  E Donchin,et al.  A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. , 1981, Science.

[16]  C. C. Duncan-Johnson Young Psychophysiologist Award address, 1980. P300 latency: a new metric of information processing. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[17]  M. Mishkin A memory system in the monkey. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[18]  E T Rolls,et al.  Neuronal responses related to visual recognition. , 1982, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  S. Zola-Morgan,et al.  Hippocampal resections impair associative learning and recognition memory in the monkey , 1982, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related potential studies of cerebral specialization during reading I. Studies of normal adults , 1982, Brain and Language.

[21]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials to grammatical errors and semantic anomalies , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[22]  D. Stuss,et al.  Event-related potentials during naming and mental rotation. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[23]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning in monkeys with medial temporal lesions: sparing of motor and cognitive skills , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and the phonological processing of words and non-words , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Anatomical and Physiological Substrates of Event‐Related Potentials , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[26]  E. Donchin,et al.  On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. , 1984, Psychophysiology.

[27]  S. Farmer,et al.  The effects of physical and semantic incongruities on auditory event-related potentials. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[28]  C. Yingling,et al.  A subcortical correlate of P300 in man. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[29]  E. Donchin,et al.  “P300” and memory: Individual differences in the von Restorff effect , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[30]  A. Starr,et al.  Intracranial potentials correlated with an event-related potential, P300, in the cat , 1985, Brain Research.

[31]  John Polich,et al.  Semantic categorization and event-related potentials , 1985, Brain and Language.

[32]  Y. Katayama,et al.  Thalamic negativity associated with the endogenous late positive component of cerebral evoked potentials (P300): Recordings using discriminative aversive conditioning in humans and cats , 1985, Brain Research Bulletin.

[33]  Mireille Besson,et al.  Visual and auditory event-related potentials elicited by linguistic and non-linguistic incongruities , 1986, Neuroscience Letters.

[34]  E. Halgren,et al.  Human medial temporal lobe potentials evoked in memory and language tasks. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[35]  E. Donchin,et al.  P300 and recall in an incidental memory paradigm. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[36]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  Apparent response incompatibility effects on P3 latency depend on the task. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  David P. Salmon,et al.  Retrograde amnesia following combined hippocampus-amygdala lesions in monkeys , 1987, Psychobiology.

[38]  M. W. Brown,et al.  Neuronal evidence that inferomedial temporal cortex is more important than hippocampus in certain processes underlying recognition memory , 1987, Brain Research.

[39]  E. Halgren,et al.  Endogenous potentials after anterior temporal lobectomy , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  J. S. Hunt,et al.  Speech event related potentials reflect linguistic content and processing level , 1987, Brain and Language.

[41]  J. Polich,et al.  Task difficulty, probability, and inter-stimulus interval as determinants of P300 from auditory stimuli. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[42]  E Halgren,et al.  Cognitive evoked potentials as modulatory processes in human memory formation and retrieval. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[43]  E Halgren,et al.  Attenuation of a sustained visual processing negativity after lesions that include the inferotemporal cortex. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[44]  R. Johnson,et al.  Scalp-recorded P300 activity in patients following unilateral temporal lobectomy. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  Masao Yukie,et al.  Direct projections from the ventral TE area of the inferotemporal cortex to hippocampal field CA1 in the monkey , 1988, Neuroscience Letters.

[46]  M. Yukie,et al.  A direct projection from hippocampal field CA1 to ventral area TE of inferotemporal cortex in the monkey , 1988, Brain Research.

[47]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Neuronal correlate of pictorial short-term memory in the primate temporal cortexYasushi Miyashita , 1988, Nature.

[48]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Dissociation of the effects of inferior temporal and limbic lesions on object discrimination learning with 24-h intertrial intervals , 1988, Behavioural Brain Research.

[49]  M. Rugg,et al.  Modulation of event-related potentials by word repetition: the effects of inter-item lag. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[50]  M. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and the semantic matching of faces , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  M. Rugg,et al.  Event-related potentials and recognition memory for words. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[52]  A. Puce,et al.  Limbic P3 potentials, seizure localization, and surgical pathology in temporal lobe epilepsy , 1989, Annals of neurology.

[53]  A. Puce,et al.  Comparative effects of age on limbic and scalp P3. , 1989, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[54]  D Friedman,et al.  Cognitive event-related potential components during continuous recognition memory for pictures. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[55]  A. Puce,et al.  Post-ictal recognition memory predicts laterality of temporal lobe seizure focus: Comparison with post-operative data , 1990, Neuropsychologia.