Generalization of action knowledge following observational learning.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J Zuckerman,et al. Modeling effects on motor performance. , 1976, Research quarterly.
[2] Dana Maslovat,et al. Observational practice benefits are limited to perceptual improvements in the acquisition of a novel coordination skill , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.
[3] Geoffrey Bird,et al. Action observation supports effector-dependent learning of finger movement sequences , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[4] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[5] Robert L. Sainburg,et al. Limitations in interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] A Bandura,et al. Translating cognition into action: the role of visual guidance in observational learning. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.
[7] G P Bingham,et al. Visual perception of the relative phasing of human limb movements , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[8] R. Sainburg. Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] Margarete Delazer,et al. Flexible transfer of knowledge in mental arithmetic — An fMRI study , 2009, NeuroImage.
[10] Simon J. Bennett,et al. Effects of manipulating relative and absolute motion information during observational learning of an aiming task , 2001, Journal of sports sciences.
[11] John J Buchanan,et al. Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice models and variability in demonstration benefits observational practice , 2010, Psychological research.
[12] R. Sainburg,et al. Differences in control of limb dynamics during dominant and nondominant arm reaching. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[13] Andrew A G Mattar,et al. Motor Learning by Observing , 2005, Neuron.
[14] Ryan K. Jessup. Transfer of high domain knowledge to a similar domain. , 2009, The American journal of psychology.
[15] Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al. Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer across different effector systems. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] John J. Buchanan,et al. Learning a single limb multijoint coordination pattern: the impact of a mechanical constraint on the coordination dynamics of learning and transfer , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Corey J Bohil,et al. Observational versus feedback training in rule-based and information-integration category learning , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[18] Natalia Dounskaia,et al. The internal model and the leading joint hypothesis: implications for control of multi-joint movements , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] J. Kelso,et al. The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics , 1994 .
[20] David L Wright,et al. Learning to Detect Error in Movement Timing Using Physical and Observational Practice , 2005, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.
[21] Albert Bandura,et al. Role of Symbolic Coding and Rehearsal Processes in Observational Learning. , 1973 .
[22] R. Sainburg,et al. Interlimb transfer of visuomotor rotations: independence of direction and final position information , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[23] Jinsung Wang,et al. Interlimb transfer of novel inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[24] John J. Buchanan,et al. Learning an environment–actor coordination skill: visuomotor transformation and coherency of perceptual structure , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[25] M. Shiffrar,et al. Subconfigurations of the human form in the perception of biological motion displays. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[26] Ian M. Franks,et al. Attention focusing instructions and coordination bias : implications for learning a novel bimanual task , 2000 .
[27] J. Kelso,et al. Evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilibrium phase transitions. , 1992 .
[28] A. Bandura. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .
[29] J. Kelso,et al. Coordination dynamics of learning and transfer: collective and component levels. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] J. Kelso,et al. Posturally induced transitions in rhythmic multijoint limb movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] Pier-Giorgio Zanone,et al. The Role of Stability in the Dynamics of Learning, Memorizing, and Forgetting New Coordination Patterns , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.
[32] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[33] Laura M. Justice,et al. Increasing the Print Focus of Adult-Child Shared Book Reading Through Observational Learning , 2000 .
[34] S. P. Swinnen,et al. Hierarchical control of different elbow-wrist coordination patterns , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] D L Wright,et al. Learning and Transfer of a Relative Phase Pattern and a Joint Amplitude Ratio in a Rhythmic Multijoint Arm Movement , 2007, Journal of motor behavior.
[36] Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al. Global Processing of Biological Motions , 1994 .
[37] G P Bingham,et al. Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase variability. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[38] Gert Rijlaarsdam,et al. Observational Learning and Its Effects on the Orchestration of Writing Processes , 2004 .
[39] S. Coren. The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: Norms for young adults , 1993 .
[40] Geoffrey Bird,et al. Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] J. A. S. Kelso,et al. Dynamic pattern generation and recognition , 1991 .
[42] J. Kelso,et al. A quantitative approach to understanding the formation and change of coordinated movement patterns. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.
[43] J. Cutting,et al. The minimum principle and the perception of absolute, common, and relative motions , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.
[44] P. Wilson,et al. Transfer of Spatial Knowledge to a Two-Level Shopping Mall in Older People, Following Virtual Exploration , 2005 .
[45] A. Opstal. Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .
[46] Ian M Franks,et al. Learning as a function of coordination bias: building upon pre-practice behaviours. , 2002, Human movement science.
[47] I M Franks,et al. Learning a Coordination Skill: Interactive Effects of Instruction and Feedback , 2001, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.
[48] Young U. Ryu,et al. Observational practice of relative but not absolute motion features in a single-limb multi-joint coordination task , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[49] J. L. Myers. Fundamentals of Experimental Design , 1972 .
[50] Romeo Chua,et al. The Role of Video in Facilitating Perception and Action of a Novel Coordination Movement , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.
[51] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[52] Polemnia G. Amazeen,et al. Is Dynamics the Content of a Generalized Motor Program for Rhythmic Interlimb Coordination? , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.
[53] S. A. Wallace,et al. Order parameters for the neural organization of single, multijoint limb movement patterns , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[54] B Abernethy,et al. Note: A statistical problem in testing invariance of movement using the phase plane model. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.
[55] Gavin Breslin,et al. Modelling relative motion to facilitate intra-limb coordination. , 2005, Human movement science.
[56] J. Kelso,et al. Learning as change of coordination dynamics: theory and experiment. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.
[57] A Bandura,et al. The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action patterns: making the unobservable observable. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.