Motor Learning of Compatible and Incompatible Visuomotor Maps

Brain imaging studies demonstrate increasing activity in limb motor areas during early motor skill learning, consistent with functional reorganization occurring at the motor output level. Nevertheless, behavioral studies reveal that visually guided skills can also be learned with respect to target location or possibly eye movements. The current experiments examined motor learning under compatible and incompatible perceptual/motor conditions to identify brain areas involved in different perceptual-motor transformations. Subjects tracked a continuously moving target with a joystick-controlled cursor. The target moved in a repeating sequence embedded within random movements to block sequence awareness. Psychophysical studies of behavioral transfer from incompatible (joystick and cursor moving in opposite directions) to compatible tracking established that incompatible learning was occurring with respect to target location. Positron emission tomography (PET) functional imaging of compatible learning identified increasing activity throughout the precentral gyrus, maximal in the arm area. Incompatible learning also led to increasing activity in the precentral gyrus, maximal in the putative frontal eye fields. When the incompatible task was switched to a compatible response and the previously learned sequence was reintroduced, there was an increase in arm motor cortex. The results show that learning-related increases of brain activity are dynamic, with recruitment of multiple motor output areas, contingent on task demands. Visually guided motor sequences can be linked to either oculomotor or arm motor areas. Rather than identifying changes of motor output maps, the data from imaging experiments may better reflect modulation of inputs to multiple motor areas.

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[2]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Motor task difficulty and brain activity: investigation of goal-directed reciprocal aiming using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Anatomy of motor learning. II. Subcortical structures and learning by trial and error. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  M. Mintun,et al.  Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Anatomy of motor learning. I. Frontal cortex and attention to action. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  J. Adams Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention, and transfer of human motor skills. , 1987 .

[7]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  A PET investigation of implicit and explicit sequence learning , 1995 .

[8]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys , 1996, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[9]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Motor subcircuits mediating the control of movement velocity: a PET study. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Functional Anatomy of a Prelearned Sequence of Horizontal Saccades in Humans , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Functional Anatomy of Visuomotor Skill Learning in Human Subjects Examined with Positron Emission Tomography , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  L. Henderson,et al.  The functional anatomy of remembered saccades: a PET study. , 1995, Neuroreport.

[14]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional Mapping of Sequence Learning in Normal Humans , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  J. Saint-Cyr,et al.  Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  R. Ammons,et al.  Acquisition of motor skill; rotary pursuit performance with continuous practice before and after a single rest. , 1947, Journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[18]  J. Haxby,et al.  Functional anatomy of pursuit eye movements in humans as revealed by fMRI. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  A. Canavan,et al.  Successive roles of the cerebellum and premotor cortices in trajectorial learning. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[20]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow during volitional breathing in man. , 1991, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  R W Pew,et al.  Levels of analysis in motor control. , 1974, Brain research.

[22]  Daniel B. Willingham,et al.  Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[23]  H. Rosenquist The Visual Response Component of Rotary Pursuit Tracking , 1965, Perceptual and motor skills.

[24]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.

[26]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modulation of cortical motor output maps during development of implicit and explicit knowledge. , 1994, Science.

[27]  D. Brooks,et al.  Motor sequence learning: a study with positron emission tomography , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[28]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Abstract and Effector-Specific Representations of Motor Sequences Identified with PET , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Kevin R. Gregg,et al.  SLA volume 17 issue 1 Cover and Front matter , 1995, Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

[30]  KM Jacobs,et al.  Reshaping the cortical motor map by unmasking latent intracortical connections , 1991, Science.

[31]  O B Paulson,et al.  The activation pattern in normal humans during suppression, imagination and performance of saccadic eye movements. , 1997, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[32]  M. Mintun,et al.  A Noninvasive Approach to Quantitative Functional Brain Mapping with H215O and Positron Emission Tomography , 1984, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[33]  T. Ebner,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of cerebellar activation during the learning of a visuomotor dissociation task , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Functional anatomy of human procedural learning determined with regional cerebral blood flow and PET , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[35]  Hiroshi Imamizu,et al.  Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool , 2000, Nature.

[36]  M. Hallett,et al.  Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Robert B. Ammons,et al.  Long-term retention of perceptual-motor skills. , 1958 .

[38]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning , 1995, Nature.

[39]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  A Noninvasive Positron Computed Tomography Technique Using Oxygen-15-Labeled Water for the Evaluation of Neurobehavioral Task Batteries , 1985, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[40]  M. Buchsbaum,et al.  Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospatial/motor task: a positron emission tomographic study , 1992, Brain Research.

[41]  J. Donoghue Plasticity of adult sensorimotor representations , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[42]  Peter Herscovitch,et al.  Brain blood flow measured with intravenous H/sub 2//sup 15/O. I. Theory and error analysis , 1983 .

[43]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Repetitive microstimulation alters the cortical representation of movements in adult rats. , 1990, Somatosensory & motor research.

[44]  C. Michel,et al.  PET study of human voluntary saccadic eye movements in darkness: effect of task repetition on the activation pattern , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[45]  Daniel B. Willingham,et al.  A Neuropsychological Theory of Motor Skill Learning , 2004 .

[46]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  M. Mintun,et al.  Brain blood flow measured with intravenous H2(15)O. II. Implementation and validation. , 1983, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[48]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Functional imaging of procedural motor learning: Relating cerebral blood flow with individual subject performance , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[49]  R. Nudo,et al.  Stimulation‐induced [14C]2‐deoxyglucose labeling of synaptic activity in the central auditory system , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[50]  E. Rawlings,et al.  Rotary Pursuit Tracking following Mental Rehearsal as a Function of Voluntary Control of Visual Imagery , 1974, Perceptual and motor skills.

[51]  G. Lynch,et al.  Memory: Organization and locus of change , 1994 .