A dynamic model of reasoning and memory.

Previous models of category-based induction have neglected how the process of induction unfolds over time. We conceive of induction as a dynamic process and provide the first fine-grained examination of the distribution of response times observed in inductive reasoning. We used these data to develop and empirically test the first major quantitative modeling scheme that simultaneously accounts for inductive decisions and their time course. The model assumes that knowledge of similarity relations among novel test probes and items stored in memory drive an accumulation-to-bound sequential sampling process: Test probes with high similarity to studied exemplars are more likely to trigger a generalization response, and more rapidly, than items with low exemplar similarity. We contrast data and model predictions for inductive decisions with a recognition memory task using a common stimulus set. Hierarchical Bayesian analyses across 2 experiments demonstrated that inductive reasoning and recognition memory primarily differ in the threshold to trigger a decision: Observers required less evidence to make a property generalization judgment (induction) than an identity statement about a previously studied item (recognition). Experiment 1 and a condition emphasizing decision speed in Experiment 2 also found evidence that inductive decisions use lower quality similarity-based information than recognition. The findings suggest that induction might represent a less cautious form of recognition. We conclude that sequential sampling models grounded in exemplar-based similarity, combined with hierarchical Bayesian analysis, provide a more fine-grained and informative analysis of the processes involved in inductive reasoning than is possible solely through examination of choice data.

[1]  S. Handley,et al.  The SDT model of belief bias: complexity, time, and cognitive ability mediate the effects of believability. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  E. Heit,et al.  Predicting Reasoning from Memory , 2022 .

[3]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  The effects of aging on the speed-accuracy compromise: Boundary optimality in the diffusion model. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[4]  E. Heit,et al.  Traditional difference-score analyses of reasoning are flawed , 2014, Cognition.

[5]  M. Lee Three case studies in the Bayesian analysis of cognitive models , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[6]  Jonathan Evans,et al.  Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning , 2005 .

[7]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.

[8]  Brett K. Hayes,et al.  Memory, reasoning, and categorization: parallels and common mechanisms , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[9]  Michael D. Lee,et al.  A Bayesian approach to diffusion process models of decision-making , 2008 .

[10]  A. Feeney,et al.  The engine of thought is a hybrid: roles of associative and structured knowledge in reasoning. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  Charles Kemp,et al.  A taxonomy of inductive problems , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[12]  A. Feeney How many processes underlie category-based induction? Effects of conclusion specificity and cognitive ability , 2007, Memory & cognition.

[13]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Structured statistical models of inductive reasoning. , 2009, Psychological review.

[14]  Evan Heit,et al.  The relationship between memory and inductive reasoning: does it develop? , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[15]  W. Loesche "The Best of Times and the Worst of Times" , 1987, Journal of dental research.

[16]  Naomi Sweller,et al.  More than one kind of inference: Re-examining what's learned in feature inference and classification , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[17]  Robert M. Nosofsky,et al.  Dissociations Between Categorization and Recognition in Amnesic and Normal Individuals: An Exemplar-Based Interpretation , 1998 .

[18]  R. Nosofsky Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986 .

[19]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[20]  E. Heit,et al.  How similar are recognition memory and inductive reasoning? , 2013, Memory & cognition.

[21]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  A ballistic model of choice response time. , 2005, Psychological review.

[22]  Russell C. Burnett,et al.  Feature inference and the causal structure of categories , 2005, Cognitive Psychology.

[23]  Bob Rehder,et al.  Category coherence and category-based property induction , 2004, Cognition.

[24]  Patrick Shafto,et al.  Effects of time pressure on context-sensitive property induction , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  Jordan J. Louviere,et al.  The Best of Times and the Worst of Times Are Interchangeable , 2014 .

[26]  Lynne M. Reder,et al.  Models of recognition: A review of arguments in favor of a dual-process account , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[27]  M. Lee How cognitive modeling can benefit from hierarchical Bayesian models. , 2011 .

[28]  Linden J. Ball,et al.  Matching bias in syllogistic reasoning: Evidence for a dual-process account from response times and confidence ratings , 2013 .

[29]  Andrew Gelman,et al.  General methods for monitoring convergence of iterative simulations , 1998 .

[30]  Brett K. Hayes,et al.  Relations Between Memory and Reasoning , 2012 .

[31]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Using best-worst scaling to improve psychological service delivery: an innovative tool for psychologists in organized care settings. , 2015, Psychological services.

[32]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  A method for efficiently sampling from distributions with correlated dimensions. , 2013, Psychological methods.

[33]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A diffusion model account of the lexical decision task. , 2004, Psychological review.

[34]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Modeling confidence judgments, response times, and multiple choices in decision making: recognition memory and motion discrimination. , 2013, Psychological review.

[35]  J. Kruschke Doing Bayesian Data Analysis , 2010 .

[36]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  The multiattribute linear ballistic accumulator model of context effects in multialternative choice. , 2014, Psychological review.

[37]  T. Zandt,et al.  A comparison of two response time models applied to perceptual matching , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[38]  Casimir J. H. Ludwig,et al.  The mechanism underlying inhibition of saccadic return , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[39]  C. Sumiyoshi CATEGORY BASED INDUCTION , 1997 .

[40]  Evan Heit,et al.  Modeling the effects of argument length and validity on inductive and deductive reasoning. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[41]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[42]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A Diffusion Model Analysis of the Effects of Aging on Recognition Memory Journal of Memory and Language , 2003 .

[43]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  The Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in the Elderly Brain: A Structural Model-Based Approach , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[45]  S. Handley,et al.  Using forced choice to test belief bias in syllogistic reasoning , 2014, Cognition.

[46]  Daniel R. Little,et al.  Activation in the neural network responsible for categorization and recognition reflects parameter changes , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[47]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Diffusion versus linear ballistic accumulation: different models for response time with different conclusions about psychological mechanisms? , 2012, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[48]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[49]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The development of category-based induction. , 1992, Child development.

[50]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. , 1997, Psychological review.

[51]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The hare and the tortoise: emphasizing speed can change the evidence used to make decisions. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  R. Nosofsky Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[53]  E. Heit,et al.  Assessing the belief bias effect with ROCs: it's a response bias effect. , 2010, Psychological review.

[54]  S. Sloman Feature-Based Induction , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[55]  Robert M. Nosofsky,et al.  An extension of the exemplar-based random-walk model to separable-dimension stimuli , 2003 .

[56]  Amy H Criss,et al.  Differentiation and response bias in episodic memory: evidence from reaction time distributions. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[57]  A. Markman,et al.  Category Learning by Inference and Classification , 1998 .

[58]  Induction , 1999 .

[59]  Richard M. Shiffrin,et al.  Familiarity and categorization processes in memory search , 2014, Cognitive Psychology.

[60]  Chris Donkin,et al.  A Power-Law Model of Psychological Memory Strength in Short- and Long-Term Recognition , 2012, Psychological science.

[61]  Evan Heit,et al.  Relations between inductive reasoning and deductive reasoning. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[62]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  When the Brain Takes a Break: A Model-Based Analysis of Mind Wandering , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[64]  J. Wixted Dual-process theory and signal-detection theory of recognition memory. , 2007, Psychological review.