Age differences in orbitofrontal activation: an fMRI investigation of delayed match and nonmatch to sample

Several investigations have suggested that the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) may be particularly vulnerable to the effects of age-related changes. We recently reported behavioral data indicating greater age differences in orbitofrontal tasks when directly compared to tasks tapping dorsolateral prefrontal functions. The present study was designed to investigate the neural underpinnings of age differences in OFC functioning. Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was performed during delayed match and nonmatch to sample tasks, previously shown to differentially activate medial and lateral OFC in young adults. Sixteen healthy younger [age = 26.7(5.6)] and 16 healthy older individuals [age = 69.1 + 5.6] with similar levels of education and general cognitive functioning participated in the experiment. Participants chose the stimulus from a pair of stimuli matching a previously viewed target (match to sample) or chose the nontarget item (nonmatch to sample) depending upon a trial-specific instruction word. Consistent with previous studies, SPM99 analyses of the younger age group revealed activation for medial OFC regions during the match task compared to the nonmatch task and lateral OFC activation during the nonmatch task compared to the match task. In contrast, older adults showed prefrontal activation only during the match relative to the nonmatch task and posterior temporal and limbic involvement during the nonmatch relative to the match task. Between-group analyses confirmed within-group results suggesting differential age-related recruitment of prefrontal regions when performing match and nonmatch tasks. Results suggest that OFC recruitment during these cognitive tasks changes with age and should be evaluated within the context of other prefrontal subregions to further define differential age effects on frontal functions.

[1]  Sarah E. MacPherson,et al.  Age, executive function, and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[2]  F. Craik,et al.  The handbook of aging and cognition , 1992 .

[3]  Adrian M. Owen,et al.  Visuo-spatial short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[4]  R. West,et al.  An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.

[5]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[6]  O. Witte,et al.  Functional Mapping of the Human Brain , 2000 .

[7]  KJ Friston Statistics I: experimental design and statistical parametric mapping , 2002 .

[9]  D. Salat,et al.  Greater orbital prefrontal volume selectively predicts worse working memory performance in older adults. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  J. A. Horel,et al.  Lesions of the anterior temporal stem and the performance of delayed match-to-sample and visual discriminations in monkeys , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[11]  R. Elliott,et al.  Differential Neural Responses during Performance of Matching and Nonmatching to Sample Tasks at Two Delay Intervals , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  Paul J. Eslinger,et al.  Orbital frontal cortex: Historical and contemdorarv views about its behavioral and physiological significanc;. An introduction to special topic papers: Part I , 1999 .

[13]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  The role of the inferior prefrontal convexity in performance of delayed nonmatching-to-sample , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Effects of orbital frontal and anterior cingulate lesions on object and spatial memory in rhesus monkeys , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Brain Mapping: The Methods , 2002 .

[16]  M. Oscar-Berman,et al.  Matching- and delayed matching-to-sample performance as measures of visual processing, selective attention, and memory in aging and alcoholic individuals , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  C. Geula,et al.  Cytoarchitecture and neural afferents of orbitofrontal cortex in the brain of the monkey , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  N. Raz Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance: Integration of structural and functional findings. , 2000 .

[19]  D. Zald,et al.  Anatomy and function of the orbital frontal cortex, II: Function and relevance to obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1996, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[20]  J. Talairach,et al.  Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging , 1988 .

[21]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[22]  J. Aggleton,et al.  The performance of amnesic subjects on tests of delayed matching-to-sample and delayed matching-to-position , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  Karl J. Friston 22 – Statistics I: Experimental Design and Statistical Parametric Mapping , 2002 .

[24]  G. Paxinos,et al.  Atlas of the Human Brain , 2000 .

[25]  Linda Teri,et al.  6/Use of the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale with Older Adults , 1986 .

[26]  Susan M Resnick,et al.  Aging and prefrontal functions: dissociating orbitofrontal and dorsolateral abilities , 2004, Neurobiology of Aging.

[27]  D. Head,et al.  Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[28]  K. Jackson,et al.  The CES-D as a screen for depression and other psychiatric disorders in adolescents. , 1991, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[29]  L. Radloff The CES-D Scale , 1977 .

[30]  R. Elliott,et al.  Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from human neuroimaging studies. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  R. Cabeza Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[32]  J. Morrison,et al.  Human orbitofrontal cortex: Cytoarchitecture and quantitative immunohistochemical parcellation , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[33]  S. Resnick,et al.  Longitudinal Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Older Adults: A Shrinking Brain , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  T G Turkington,et al.  Adult age differences in the functional neuroanatomy of verbal recognition memory , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[35]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  Visual recognition impairment follows ventromedial but not dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in monkeys , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  Oliver T. Wolf,et al.  Volumetric analysis of the pre-frontal regions: findings in aging and schizophrenia , 2001, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[38]  E. Rolls The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  G. McCarthy,et al.  The Effects of Aging upon the Hemodynamic Response Measured by Functional MRI , 2001, NeuroImage.

[40]  S. Resnick,et al.  One-year age changes in MRI brain volumes in older adults. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.