Saccadic localization of random dot targets

The targets for saccadic eye movements in natural visual scenes are spatially extended objects, yet saccades land at a single position within them. To characterize the spatial transformation that determines the saccadic goal position within attended objects, we studied saccadic localization of large patterns of random dots. Saccades landed with a high degree of precision near the center-of-gravity of the patterns (average error < 10%; SDs around the center-of-gravity = 7-11% of target eccentricity). Predictions of landing position were improved by using a weighted center-of-gravity, in which the weight assigned to each dot was reduced by the presence of neighboring dots. Weighting based either on the eccentricity of dots or their position relative to the boundary of the pattern had no effect. The results can be accounted for by a spatial transformation in which the "local signs" of an initial array of detectors, weighted by the activity of each, are averaged to yield the saccadic goal. This model can account for accurate and precise saccadic localization of large targets, while preserving sensitivity to local pattern characteristics. Unlike models of recognition, the boundary of the object has the same status as the internal details.

[1]  Erik Blaser,et al.  The accuracy and precision of saccades to small and large targets , 1995, Vision Research.

[2]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The visual and frontal cortices. , 1989, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[3]  C. A. Burbeck,et al.  Scaled position integration areas: accounting for Weber’s law for separation , 1993 .

[4]  D M Levi,et al.  Spatial-interval discrimination in the human fovea: what delimits the interval? , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[5]  J. Stern Theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research A. G. Gale and F. Johnson, (Elsevier Science Publishers B.V., Amsterdam, 1984) pp. xiii + 565, Dfl. 185 , 1985, Biological Psychology.

[6]  D. Sparks,et al.  Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus , 1988, Nature.

[7]  A. A. Skavenski,et al.  Miniature eye movement. , 1973, Science.

[8]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The role of location probability in the programming of saccades: Implications for “center-of-gravity” tendencies , 1989, Vision Research.

[9]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—IV. Anticipatory smooth eye movements depend on prior target motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[10]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[11]  Suzanne P. McKee,et al.  Integration regions for visual hyperacuity , 1977, Vision Research.

[12]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—I. Periodic target steps , 1979, Vision Research.

[13]  E Kowler,et al.  Saccadic localization of eccentric forms. , 1991, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[14]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[15]  Z. Kapoula,et al.  Evidence for a range effect in the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.

[16]  Christian Coëffé,et al.  Reducing the influence of non-target stimuli on saccade accuracy: Predictability and latency effects , 1987, Vision Research.

[17]  Eric Mjolsness,et al.  A center-of-mass computation describes the precision of random dot displacement discrimination , 1992, Vision Research.

[18]  S. Coren,et al.  Effect of Non-Target Stimuli upon Length of Voluntary Saccades , 1972, Perceptual and motor skills.

[19]  Nadejda Bocheva,et al.  Perceived location of two-dimensional patterns , 1993, Vision Research.

[20]  David Badcock,et al.  Localization of element clusters by the human visual system , 1994, Vision Research.

[21]  W. Wolf,et al.  The Evaluation of the Oculomotor Error Signal , 1984 .

[22]  ROBERT F. HESS,et al.  Primitives Used in the Spatial Localization of Nonabutting Stimuli: Peaks or Centroids , 1996, Vision Research.

[23]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  D. Levi,et al.  Spatial localization without visual references , 1992, Vision Research.

[25]  H. D. Crane,et al.  Accurate three-dimensional eyetracker. , 1978, Applied optics.

[26]  K. M. L. Suxena,et al.  Introduction to Statistical Theory , 1976 .

[27]  J. Findlay Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.

[28]  J. V. Gisbergen,et al.  Collicular ensemble coding of saccades based on vector summation , 1987, Neuroscience.

[29]  J. O'regan,et al.  Eye fixations near corners: evidence for a centre of gravity calculation based on contrast, rather than luminance or curvature , 1994, Vision Research.

[30]  M. Morgan,et al.  Biases and sensitivities in geometrical illusions , 1990, Vision Research.

[31]  John M. Findlay,et al.  The spatial signal for saccadic eye movements emphasizes visual boundaries , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  F. Ottes,et al.  Latency dependence of colour-based target vs nontarget discrimination by the saccadic system , 1985, Vision Research.