Time and time again

In the study of human-machine systems, the need to have a model of the user is by now taken for granted. The model can be used both as support for design and analysis and as a representation of the user that resides somewhere in the machine. When it comes to the practice of modelling, two characteristic approaches can be recognized. The first focuses on the how of modelling, and is concerned mainly with the structure and contents of models. The second focuses on what is being modelled, and is concerned mainly with the functioning or performance of the model. The first approach has dominated human-machine systems research for several decades, and has led to orthodoxy in modelling by which certain structural characteristics are accepted without questioning. This unreflective attitude to modelling has been criticised several times, although with little effect. In taking the second approach and focusing on what should be modelled, two important issues are that human performance varies in level of control, i.e. in terms of how orderly it is, and that thinking and acting take time-- and occur in a context where time is limited. Although it is clearly essential that user models can account for these characteristics, very few existing models are capable of doing so because they focus on internal information processing rather than on performance in a dynamic environment. The paper describes a type of functional model, called contextual control models, which shows how it is possible to account for both different control modes and how performance is affected by time. Indeed, control and time are intimately linked and loss of one may lead to a loss of the other. The contextual control model distinguishes among four characteristic control modes (strategic, tactical, opportunistic and scrambled) and two time parameters (time to evaluate, time to select) that are seen relative to the available time. Finally, a number of applications of contextual control models are described.

[1]  M. S. Mayzner,et al.  Cognition And Reality , 1976 .

[2]  Timothy R. Colburn Defeasible reasoning and logic programming , 1991, Minds and Machines.

[3]  F. Decortis,et al.  TIME: THE CINDERELLA OF MAN-MACHINE INTERACTION , 1988 .

[4]  Hiroshi Furukawa,et al.  Situation-adaptive interface based on abstraction hierarchies with an updating mechanism for maintaining situation awareness of plant operators , 1999, IEEE SMC'99 Conference Proceedings. 1999 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (Cat. No.99CH37028).

[5]  Erik Hollnagel,et al.  Cognitive Systems Engineering: New Wine in New Bottles , 1983, Int. J. Man Mach. Stud..

[6]  Erik Hollnagel,et al.  Human Reliability Analysis: Context and Control , 1994 .

[7]  Kim J. Vicente,et al.  Supporting operator problem solving through ecological interface design , 1995, IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern..

[8]  J. Shaoul Human Error , 1973, Nature.

[9]  Neville A. Stanton,et al.  Testing Hollnagel's Contextual Control Model: Assessing Team Behavior in a Human Supervisory Control Task , 2001 .

[10]  Sidney W. A. Dekker,et al.  To Intervene or not to Intervene: The Dilemma of Management by Exception , 1999, Cognition, Technology & Work.

[11]  Philip N. Johnson-Laird,et al.  Mental Models in Cognitive Science , 1980, Cogn. Sci..

[12]  David H. Weir,et al.  New Results in Driver Steering Control Models , 1977 .

[13]  Erik Hollnagel,et al.  Human–machine function allocation: a functional modelling approach , 1999 .

[14]  Timothy R. Colburn Program verification, defeasible reasoning, and two views of computer science , 1991, Minds and Machines.

[15]  G Volta Time and decision , 1986 .

[16]  Jens Rasmussen,et al.  Information Processing and Human-Machine Interaction: An Approach to Cognitive Engineering , 1986 .

[17]  W. Ashby,et al.  Every Good Regulator of a System Must Be a Model of That System , 1970 .

[18]  F. Donders On the speed of mental processes. , 1969, Acta psychologica.

[19]  J. L. Alty,et al.  Multimedia interface design in process control , 1992 .