Phonological processing of nonwords by a deep dyslexic patient: A rowse is implicitly a rose
暂无分享,去创建一个
L. Buchanan | N. Hildebrandt | G. E. Mackinnon | L. Buchanan | N. Hildebrandt | G. E. MacKinnon | Nancy Hildebrandt
[1] R. Friedman,et al. The Continuum of Deep/Phonological Alexia , 1990, Cortex.
[2] A. Caramazza,et al. Where Do Semantic Errors Come From? , 1990, Cortex.
[3] E K Warrington,et al. Concrete word dyslexia. , 1981, British journal of psychology.
[4] T. Shallice,et al. Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .
[5] R. Schvaneveldt,et al. Functions of graphemic and phonemic codes in visual word-recognition , 1974, Memory & cognition.
[6] K. Paap,et al. Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race , 1991 .
[7] D. Besner,et al. Reading Aloud: Evidence for the Use of a Whole Word Nonsemantic Pathway , 1993 .
[8] Mark S. Seidenberg,et al. Spelling-sound effects in reading: Time-course and decision criteria , 1985, Memory & cognition.
[9] Neil A. Macmillan,et al. Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .
[10] Max Coltheart,et al. Access to the internal lexicon , 1977 .
[11] G. Stone,et al. Strategic control of processing in word recognition. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] D. Caplan. Toward a Psycholinguistic Approach to Acquired Neurogenic Language Disorders , 1993 .
[13] E. Warrington,et al. New Method of Testing Long-term Retention with Special Reference to Amnesic Patients , 1968, Nature.
[14] T Shallice,et al. Semantic access dyslexia. , 1979, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[15] James L. McClelland,et al. A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.
[16] N. Kapur,et al. Recognition Reading in Paralexia , 1978, Cortex.
[17] Implicit sublexical phonological processing in an acquired dyslexic patient , 1993 .
[18] L. Katz,et al. Evidence of flexible coding in visual word recognition. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] K. Patterson. Lexical but nonsemantic spelling , 1986 .
[20] Derek Besner,et al. Is the right hemisphere literate , 1984 .
[21] E. Warrington,et al. Amnesic Syndrome: Consolidation or Retrieval? , 1970, Nature.
[22] Randi C. Martin,et al. The Pseudohomophone Effect: The Role of Visual Similarity in Non-Word Decisions , 1982, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[23] Phonemic Dyslexia: Errors of Meaning and the Meaning of Errors , 1978, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[24] Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al. Lesioning an attractor network: investigations of acquired dyslexia , 1991 .
[25] M. S. Mayzner,et al. Tables of single-letter and digram frequency counts for various word-length and letter-position combinations. , 1965 .
[26] S. Lanzoni,et al. Automatic Activation of Word Phonology from Print in Deep Dyslexia , 1992 .
[27] Christopher Barry,et al. Accounts of oral reading in deep dyslexia , 1988 .
[28] John C. Marshall,et al. Deep dyslexia since 1980. , 1987 .
[29] A. Young,et al. Face recognition without awareness , 1987 .
[30] James L. McClelland,et al. Generalization with Componential Attractors: Word and Nonword Reading in an Attractor Network , 1993 .
[31] G. Waters,et al. An analysis of the spelling errors of children who differ in their reading and spelling skills , 1988, Applied Psycholinguistics.
[32] A. J. Marcel,et al. Aphasia, Dyslexia and the Phonological Coding of Written Words , 1977 .
[33] M. Turvey,et al. Phonological access of the lexicon: evidence from associative priming with pseudohomophones. , 1991 .
[34] R. Friedman,et al. Impaired activation of the phonological lexicon: Effects upon oral reading , 1990, Brain and Language.
[35] Randall C. O'Reilly,et al. Dissociated overt and covert recognition as an emergent property of a lesioned neural network. , 1993, Psychological review.