Sex differences in emoji use, familiarity, and valence

Abstract Emojis (particularly smiley emojis, ☺) are increasingly used in computer-mediated communication as well as in applied domains within marketing, healthcare, and psychology. The emotional negativity bias in the facial emotion processing literature posits that women are more sensitive to negative facial emotion than are men. Given the similarity in neural processing between human faces and smiley emojis, women may likewise view negative smiley emojis as more negative than do men. Moreover, the familiarity of the emoji and the participants' overall emoji use may increase the positivity of the emoji. To investigate these potential influences of sex, familiarity, and emoji use on the valence of smiley emojis, we assessed the familiarity and the perceived valence for 70 iOS facial emojis in a large sample (N = 299; 163 women) of United States college students (Mage = 19.66, SDage = 2.72). Results indicated higher emoji usage and familiarity ratings for women than for men. In assessing valence we found higher overall positive ratings for men than for women. Consistent with the emotional negativity bias, this sex difference was limited to the negative smiley emojis with no sex difference in valence for the positive emojis. The obtained sex differences in smiley emojis’ use, familiarity, and valence are an important consideration in the selection of such stimuli in future studies.

[1]  David Matsumoto,et al.  Judgments of facial expressions of emotion in profile. , 2011, Emotion.

[2]  Daantje Derks,et al.  Emoticons and social interaction on the Internet: the importance of social context , 2007, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[3]  Xuanzhe Liu,et al.  Through a Gender Lens: Learning Usage Patterns of Emojis from Large-Scale Android Users , 2017, WWW.

[4]  James A. Roberts,et al.  The invisible addiction: Cell-phone activities and addiction among male and female college students , 2014, Journal of behavioral addictions.

[5]  Miguel Sotaquira,et al.  Cortical processing of human and emoji faces: an ERP analysis , 2020, Behav. Inf. Technol..

[6]  F. Giannotta,et al.  “Development and preliminary validation of an image-based instrument to assess depressive symptoms” , 2019, Psychiatry Research.

[7]  Marianne Swaney‐Stueve,et al.  The emoji scale: A facial scale for the 21st century , 2018, Food Quality and Preference.

[8]  Daniel Voyer,et al.  Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: A meta-analysis , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[9]  T. Suslow,et al.  Ruminative response style is associated with a negative bias in the perception of emotional facial expressions in healthy women without a history of clinical depression. , 2019, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[10]  J. Hewig,et al.  Do emojis influence social interactions? Neural and behavioral responses to affective emojis in bargaining situations. , 2019, Psychophysiology.

[11]  Stephan Hamann,et al.  Sex differences in brain activation to emotional stimuli: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  J. Thayer,et al.  Trait Anxiety Is Associated with Negative Interpretations When Resolving Valence Ambiguity of Surprised Faces , 2016, Frontiers in Psychology.

[13]  Marília Prada,et al.  A frown emoji can be worth a thousand words: Perceptions of emoji use in text messages exchanged between romantic partners , 2017, Telematics Informatics.

[14]  J. Verwaeren,et al.  Emoji as a tool for measuring children’s emotions when tasting food , 2018, Food Quality and Preference.

[15]  David I. Perrett,et al.  Sex differences in the perception of affective facial expressions: Do men really lack emotional sensitivity? , 2005, Cognitive Processing.

[16]  J. Yuille,et al.  Individual differences in empathy: The role of facial expression recognition , 2010 .

[17]  Marília Prada,et al.  Lisbon Symbol Database (LSD): Subjective norms for 600 symbols , 2016, Behavior research methods.

[18]  D. Sauter,et al.  Commonalities outweigh differences in the communication of emotions across human cultures [Letter to the editor] , 2013 .

[19]  Ed Diener,et al.  Affect intensity and reactions to daily life events. , 1986 .

[20]  D. Bowers,et al.  Differences in startle reactivity during the perception of angry and fearful faces. , 2007, Emotion.

[21]  G. Wilson,et al.  Sex, sexual orientation, and identification of positive and negative facial affect , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[22]  Clayton Shepard,et al.  A longitudinal study of emoticon use in text messaging from smartphones , 2012, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[23]  S. Shirk,et al.  Subtypes of Rumination in Adolescence: Associations Between Brooding, Reflection, Depressive Symptoms, and Coping , 2007, Journal of clinical child and adolescent psychology : the official journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53.

[24]  J. Adelman,et al.  Automatic vigilance for negative words in lexical decision and naming: comment on Larsen, Mercer, and Balota (2006). , 2008, Emotion.

[25]  Anette Kersting,et al.  Women's Greater Ability to Perceive Happy Facial Emotion Automatically: Gender Differences in Affective Priming , 2012, PloS one.

[26]  G. J. Lewis,et al.  Sex Differences in Emotion Recognition: Evidence for a Small Overall Female Superiority on Facial Disgust , 2019, Emotion.

[27]  O. John,et al.  Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  Marília Prada,et al.  Lisbon Emoji and Emoticon Database (LEED): Norms for emoji and emoticons in seven evaluative dimensions , 2017, Behavior Research Methods.

[29]  Amy Beth Warriner,et al.  Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas , 2013, Behavior Research Methods.

[30]  R. Elliott,et al.  Cognitive Mechanisms of Treatment in Depression , 2012, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[31]  Amy Beth Warriner,et al.  Emotion and language: valence and arousal affect word recognition. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[32]  Gopal Das,et al.  To emoji or not to emoji? Examining the influence of emoji on consumer reactions to advertising , 2019, Journal of Business Research.

[33]  Heather M. Claypool,et al.  Familiar eyes are smiling: On the role of familiarity in the perception of facial affect , 2007 .

[34]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  The Empathy Quotient: An Investigation of Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and Normal Sex Differences , 2004, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[35]  Zachary Estes,et al.  Automatic vigilance for negative words is categorical and general , 2008 .

[36]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. , 2010, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[37]  D. Skiba,et al.  Face with Tears of Joy Is Word of the Year: Are Emoji a Sign of Things to Come in Health Care? , 2016, Nursing education perspectives.

[38]  Linda K. Kaye,et al.  "Turn that frown upside-down": A contextual account of emoticon usage on different virtual platforms , 2016, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[39]  L. Abramson,et al.  Gender Differences in Depression in Representative National Samples: Meta-Analyses of Diagnoses and Symptoms , 2017, Psychological bulletin.

[40]  RodriguesDavid,et al.  A frown emoji can be worth a thousand words , 2017 .

[41]  Petra Kralj Novak,et al.  Sentiment of Emojis , 2015, PloS one.

[42]  Lucy I Mullin,et al.  A female advantage in the recognition of emotional facial expressions: test of an evolutionary hypothesis , 2006 .

[43]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .

[44]  Douglas A. Vakoch,et al.  Speech perception and vocal expression of emotion , 2001 .

[45]  F. Giannotta,et al.  Assessing personality using emoji: An exploratory study , 2017 .

[46]  Manuel Perea,et al.  ERP correlates of masked affective priming with emoticons 1-s2.0-S0747563212003007-fx1 , 2013, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[47]  Simon Surguladze,et al.  Empathy costs: Negative emotional bias in high empathisers , 2015, Psychiatry Research.

[48]  M. Garrido,et al.  Comparing the valence, emotionality and subjective familiarity of words in a first and a second language , 2018 .

[49]  R. Zajonc Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .

[50]  L. Carstensen,et al.  The positivity effect: a negativity bias in youth fades with age , 2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[51]  Monica A. Riordan Emojis as Tools for Emotion Work: Communicating Affect in Text Messages , 2017 .

[52]  Nicole C. Krämer,et al.  Same Same But Different!? The Differential Influence of Smilies and Emoticons on Person Perception , 2012, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[53]  L. Galea,et al.  Sex differences in depression: Insights from clinical and preclinical studies , 2019, Progress in Neurobiology.

[54]  Andrea Hildebrandt,et al.  Sex differences in facial emotion perception ability across the lifespan , 2019, Cognition & emotion.

[55]  A. Woodward,et al.  Not all emotions are created equal: the negativity bias in social-emotional development. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.

[56]  Maureen A. Coyle,et al.  Perceived responsiveness in text messaging: The role of emoji use , 2019, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[57]  DAANTJE DERKS,et al.  Emoticons in Computer-Mediated Communication: Social Motives and Social Context , 2008, Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw..

[58]  S. Jaeger,et al.  Can emoji be used as a direct method to measure emotional associations to food names? Preliminary investigations with consumers in USA and China , 2017 .

[59]  N. Yahata,et al.  Neural basis of negativity bias in the perception of ambiguous facial expression , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[60]  Ulrike Gretzel,et al.  Emoji rhetoric: a social media influencer perspective , 2018, Journal of Marketing Management.

[61]  M. Kret,et al.  A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[62]  Benny B. Briesemeister,et al.  Avoid violence, rioting, and outrage; approach celebration, delight, and strength: Using large text corpora to compute valence, arousal, and the basic emotions , 2015, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[63]  Lisa J. Orchard,et al.  Emoticon convergence in Internet chat rooms , 2013 .

[64]  A. Proverbio Sex differences in social cognition: The case of face processing , 2017, Journal of neuroscience research.

[65]  Lee H. Wurm,et al.  Decreasing complexity of affective space in older adults lower on cognitive control: affective effects in a nonaffective task and with nonaffective stimuli. , 2011, Psychology and aging.

[66]  P. Ekman,et al.  DIFFERENCES Universals and Cultural Differences in the Judgments of Facial Expressions of Emotion , 2004 .

[67]  G. A. Kleef,et al.  The Dark Side of a Smiley , 2018 .

[68]  Xuanzhe Liu,et al.  Through a Gender Lens: Learning Usage Patterns of Emojis from Large-Scale Android Users , 2017, WWW.

[69]  Naomi G. Rotter,et al.  Sex differences in the encoding and decoding of negative facial emotions , 1988 .

[70]  Oliver G. B. Garrod,et al.  Facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[71]  Shuang Liu,et al.  Do pictures help tell the story? An experimental test of narrative and emojis in a health text message intervention , 2018, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[72]  E. McClure A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[73]  Marília Prada,et al.  Motives, frequency and attitudes toward emoji and emoticon use , 2018, Telematics Informatics.

[74]  David A. Lishner,et al.  Addressing measurement limitations in affective rating scales: Development of an empirical valence scale , 2008 .