On the origins of logarithmic number-to-position mapping.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Number line compression and the illusory perception of random numbers. , 2010, Experimental psychology.
[2] Marco Zorzi,et al. Selective impairment for reading numbers and number words: a single case study , 2004, Neuropsychologia.
[3] R. Bakeman. Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs , 2005, Behavior research methods.
[4] Cornelius Weiller,et al. “I know your name, but not your number” – Patients with verbal short-term memory deficits are impaired in learning sequences of digits , 2015, Neuropsychologia.
[5] R. Siegler,et al. The Development of Numerical Estimation , 2003, Psychological science.
[6] M. Desmurget,et al. An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[7] Ken Nakayama,et al. Numeric comparison in a visually-guided manual reaching task , 2008, Cognition.
[8] J. Algina,et al. Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. , 2003, Psychological methods.
[9] S. Dehaene,et al. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .
[10] Marco Zorzi,et al. Numerical estimation in preschoolers. , 2010, Developmental psychology.
[11] Pierre Jolicœur,et al. All-or-none bottleneck versus capacity sharing accounts of the psychological refractory period phenomenon , 2002, Psychological research.
[12] S Dehaene,et al. The psychophysics of numerical comparison: A reexamination of apparently incompatible data , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[13] H Pashler,et al. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] R. Núñez,et al. Squeezing, striking, and vocalizing: Is number representation fundamentally spatial? , 2011, Cognition.
[15] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[16] S Dehaene,et al. Competition between past and present. Assessment and interpretation of verbal perseverations. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[17] W. Fias,et al. “I can write seven but I can’t say it”: a case of domain-specific phonological output deficit for numbers , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[18] J. Houk,et al. Deciding when and how to correct a movement: discrete submovements as a decision making process , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] D. Whitney,et al. Serial dependence in visual perception , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[20] Emmanuel Dupoux,et al. Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990 .
[21] L. Dehaene,et al. Neologistic Jargon Sparing Numbers: A Category-specific Phonological Impairment , 1997 .
[22] S. Dehaene. Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation , 2007 .
[23] Florent Meyniel,et al. The Sense of Confidence during Probabilistic Learning: A Normative Account , 2015, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[24] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[25] Marinella Cappelletti,et al. Dissociations in numerical abilities revealed by progressive cognitive decline in a patient with semantic dementia , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[26] M. Brysbaert. Arabic number reading: On the nature of the numerical scale and the origin of phonological recoding. , 1995 .
[27] D. Burr,et al. Compressive mapping of number to space reflects dynamic encoding mechanisms, not static logarithmic transform , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[28] Michael N. Shadlen,et al. Temporal context calibrates interval timing , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.
[29] Alfonso Caramazza,et al. Engaging the motor system with masked orthographic primes: A kinematic analysis , 2008 .
[30] L. Maloney,et al. Decision-theoretic models of visual perception and action , 2010, Vision Research.
[31] W. Schwarz,et al. The perception of temporal order along the mental number line. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[32] M. Sigman,et al. Dynamics of the Central Bottleneck: Dual-Task and Task Uncertainty , 2006, PLoS biology.
[33] S. Dehaene,et al. Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[34] Thomas A. Farmer,et al. Hand in Motion Reveals Mind in Motion , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[35] Julie L. Booth,et al. Development of numerical estimation in young children. , 2004, Child development.
[36] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Decoding the Dynamics of Action, Intention, and Error Detection for Conscious and Subliminal Stimuli , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] Jason Friedman,et al. Linking cognitive and reaching trajectories via intermittent movement control , 2013 .
[38] D. J. Simpson,et al. Pointing the way , 1993, Nature.
[39] Susana Ruiz Fernández,et al. Number magnitude determines gaze direction: Spatial–numerical association in a free-choice task , 2011, Cortex.
[40] S Dehaene,et al. CALCULATING WITHOUT READING: UNSUSPECTED RESIDUAL ABILITIES IN PURE ALEXIA , 2000, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[41] J. Friedman,et al. The Flexibility of Nonconsciously Deployed Cognitive Processes: Evidence from Masked Congruence Priming , 2011, PloS one.
[42] Philippe Pinel,et al. Tuning Curves for Approximate Numerosity in the Human Intraparietal Sulcus , 2004, Neuron.
[43] Naama Friedmann,et al. Steps towards understanding the phonological output buffer and its role in the production of numbers, morphemes, and function words , 2015, Cortex.
[44] M. Coltheart,et al. Pointing the way to new constraints on the dynamical claims of computational models. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[45] J. Cantlon,et al. Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans , 2006, Psychological science.
[46] Samuel Shaki,et al. Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[47] Tom Verguts,et al. Distance in Motion: Response Trajectories Reveal the Dynamics of Number Comparison , 2011, PloS one.
[48] M von Aster. Developmental cognitive neuropsychology of number processing and calculation: varieties of developmental dyscalculia. , 2000, European child & adolescent psychiatry.
[49] Emmanuel Dupoux,et al. AN INFLUENCE OF SYNTACTIC AND SEMANTIC VARIABLES ON WORD FORM RETRIEVAL , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[50] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Two mental calculation systems: A case study of severe acalculia with preserved approximation , 1991, Neuropsychologia.
[51] Robert S. Siegler,et al. Representational change and children’s numerical estimation , 2007, Cognitive Psychology.
[52] D. Wolpert,et al. Changing your mind: a computational mechanism of vacillation , 2009, Nature.
[53] John Opfer,et al. Development of Numerosity Estimation: A Linear to Logarithmic Shift? , 2015, CogSci.
[54] M. Sigman,et al. Parsing a Cognitive Task: A Characterization of the Mind's Bottleneck , 2005, PLoS biology.
[55] H. Barth,et al. Developmental change in numerical estimation. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[56] Jeffrey N Rouder,et al. Children's cognitive representation of the mathematical number line. , 2014, Developmental science.
[57] Robert S. Siegler,et al. Development of Numerical Estimation , 2004 .
[58] Melissa E. Libertus,et al. Comment on "Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures" , 2009, Science.
[59] T. Flash,et al. The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[60] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. How do we convert a number into a finger trajectory? , 2013, Cognition.
[61] Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.
[62] D. Algom,et al. Size congruity effects with two-digit numbers: Expanding the number line? , 2006, Memory & cognition.
[63] E. Spelke,et al. Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.
[64] Julie L. Booth,et al. Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. , 2006, Developmental psychology.
[65] Yong-chun Cai,et al. The effect of numerical magnitude on the perceptual processing speed of a digit. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[66] K. Nakayama,et al. Hidden cognitive states revealed in choice reaching tasks , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[67] Stella F. Lourenco,et al. Multiple spatial representations of number: evidence for co-existing compressive and linear scales , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[68] K. Nakayama,et al. Target selection in visual search as revealed by movement trajectories , 2008, Vision Research.
[69] Hilary C Barth,et al. The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. , 2011, Developmental science.
[70] J. Gold,et al. Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[71] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Cognitive euroscience: Scalar variability in price estimation and the cognitive consequences of switching to the euro , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[72] Naama Friedmann,et al. Breaking down number syntax: Spared comprehension of multi-digit numbers in a patient with impaired digit-to-word conversion , 2014, Cortex.
[73] E. Miller,et al. Coding of Cognitive Magnitude Compressed Scaling of Numerical Information in the Primate Prefrontal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.
[74] David C. Burr,et al. Linear mapping of numbers onto space requires attention , 2012, Cognition.