Who or What to Believe: Trust and the Differential Persuasiveness of Human and Anthropomorphized Messengers

Participants in three studies read advertisements in which messages were delivered either by people or by anthropomorphized agents—specifically, “talking” products. The results indicate that people low in interpersonal trust are more persuaded by anthropomorphized messengers than by human spokespeople because low trusters are more attentive to the nature of the messenger and believe that humans, more than partial humans (i.e., anthropomorphized agents), lack goodwill. People high in interpersonal trust are less attentive about who is trying to persuade them and so respond similarly to human and anthropomorphized messengers. However, when prompted to be attentive, they are more persuaded by human spokespeople than by anthropomorphized messengers due to their belief that humans, more than partial humans, act with goodwill. Under conditions in which attentiveness is low for all consumers, high and low trusters alike are unaffected by the nature of persuasion agents. The authors discuss the implications of the findings for advertisers considering the use of anthropomorphized “spokespeople.”

[1]  F. L. Ruch,et al.  Psychology and Life , 2001 .

[2]  J. Rotter Social learning and clinical psychology , 2017 .

[3]  Sara Kim,et al.  Gaming with Mr. Slot or Gaming the Slot Machine? Power, Anthropomorphism, and Risk Perception , 2011 .

[4]  J. Mccroskey,et al.  Goodwill: A reexamination of the construct and its measurement , 1999 .

[5]  H. E. Krugman THE IMPACT OF TELEVISION ADVERTISING: LEARNING WITHOUT INVOLVEMENT , 1965 .

[6]  Jan R. Landwehr,et al.  It's Got the Look: The Effect of Friendly and Aggressive “Facial” Expressions on Product Liking and Sales , 2011 .

[7]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Creating Social Connection Through Inferential Reproduction , 2008, Psychological science.

[8]  Norbert Schwarz,et al.  Use does not wear ragged the fabric of friendship: Thinking of objects as alive makes people less willing to replace them , 2010 .

[9]  Peter Wright,et al.  Persuasion Knowledge , 2022 .

[10]  L. Phillips,et al.  The persuasive effect of source credibility: a situational analysis. , 1978, Public opinion quarterly.

[11]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[12]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. , 2007, Psychological review.

[13]  John B. Campbell,et al.  Dimensionality of the Rotter Interpersonal Trust Scale , 1974 .

[14]  J. Neyman,et al.  Tests of certain linear hypotheses and their application to some educational problems. , 1936 .

[15]  A. Bandura Social learning theory , 1977 .

[16]  N. Haslam,et al.  Understanding the Relationship between Attribute-Based and Metaphor-Based Dehumanization , 2009 .

[17]  Robert R. Harmon,et al.  The Persuasive Effects of Source Credibility in Buy and Lease Situations , 1982 .

[18]  P. Aggarwal,et al.  When Brands Seem Human, Do Humans Act Like Brands? Automatic Behavioral Priming Effects of Brand Anthropomorphism , 2012 .

[19]  John G. Lynch,et al.  Spotlights, Floodlights, and the Magic Number Zero: Simple Effects Tests in Moderated Regression , 2012 .

[20]  R. Gerrig,et al.  心理学与生活=Psychology and life , 2003 .

[21]  D. Wegner,et al.  Dimensions of Mind Perception , 2007, Science.

[22]  Heloir,et al.  The Uncanny Valley , 2019, The Animation Studies Reader.

[23]  J. Rotter A new scale for the measurement of interpersonal trust. , 1967, Journal of personality.

[24]  Amna Kirmani,et al.  Consumers' Use of Persuasion Knowledge: The Effects of Accessibility and Cognitive Capacity on Perceptions of an Influence Agent , 2000 .

[25]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Personal involvement as a determinant of argument based persuasion , 1981 .

[26]  P. Aggarwal,et al.  Is That Car Smiling at Me? Schema Congruity as a Basis for Evaluating Anthropomorphized Products , 2007 .

[27]  S. Guthrie,et al.  Faces in the Clouds: A New Theory of Religion , 1994 .

[28]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  WHEN WE NEED A HUMAN: MOTIVATIONAL DETERMINANTS OF ANTHROPOMORPHISM , 2008 .

[29]  John G. Lynch,et al.  Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and Truths about Mediation Analysis , 2010 .

[30]  B. Sternthal,et al.  Highly Credible Sources: Persuasive Facilitators or Persuasive Liabilities? , 1977 .

[31]  Hee-Kyung Ahn,et al.  Helping Fellow Beings , 2014, Psychological science.

[32]  H. Kelley,et al.  Communication and Persuasion: Psychological Studies of Opinion Change , 1982 .

[33]  M. Gurtman,et al.  Interpersonal Trust and Perceptual Vigilance for Trustworthiness Descriptors. , 1982 .

[34]  Richard L. Celsi,et al.  The Role of Involvement in Attention and Comprehension Processes , 1988 .

[35]  Eric R. Spangenberg,et al.  Development of a Scale to Measure Consumer Skepticism Toward Advertising , 1998 .

[36]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Issue involvement can increase or decrease persuasion by enhancing message-relevant cognitive responses. , 1979 .

[37]  R. Tedeschi,et al.  Factor anlaysis of the Interpersonal Trust Scale. , 1975 .

[38]  Andrew F. Hayes,et al.  Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations , 2009, Behavior research methods.