Digital Clock Drawing: Differentiating “Thinking” versus “Doing” in Younger and Older Adults with Depression

Abstract Psychomotor slowing has been documented in depression. The digital Clock Drawing Test (dCDT) provides: (i) a novel technique to assess both cognitive and motor aspects of psychomotor speed within the same task and (ii) the potential to uncover subtleties of behavior not previously detected with non-digitized modes of data collection. Using digitized pen technology in 106 participants grouped by Age (younger/older) and Affect (euthymic/unmedicated depressed), we recorded cognitive and motor output by capturing how the clock is drawn rather than focusing on the final product. We divided time to completion (TTC) for Command and Copy conditions of the dCDT into metrics of percent of drawing (%Ink) versus non-drawing (%Think) time. We also obtained composite Z-scores of cognition, including attention/information processing (AIP), to explore associations of %Ink and %Think times to cognitive and motor performance. Despite equivalent TTC, %Ink and %Think Command times (Copy n.s.) were significant (AgeXAffect interaction: p=.03)—younger depressed spent a smaller proportion of time drawing relative to thinking compared to the older depressed group. Command %Think time negatively correlated with AIP in the older depressed group (r=−.46; p=.02). Copy %Think time negatively correlated with AIP in the younger depressed (r=−.47; p=.03) and older euthymic groups (r=−.51; p=.01). The dCDT differentiated aspects of psychomotor slowing in depression regardless of age, while dCDT/cognitive associates for younger adults with depression mimicked patterns of older euthymics. (JINS, 2014, 20, 1–9)

[1]  Wouter Hulstijn,et al.  Psychomotor symptoms in depression: a diagnostic, pathophysiological and therapeutic tool. , 2008, Journal of affective disorders.

[2]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[3]  J. Sneed,et al.  Executive dysfunction and treatment response in late‐life depression , 2012, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[4]  R B D'Agostino,et al.  Probability of stroke: a risk profile from the Framingham Study. , 1991, Stroke.

[5]  M. First,et al.  The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID). I: History, rationale, and description. , 1992, Archives of general psychiatry.

[6]  J. J. Ryan,et al.  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III , 2001 .

[7]  J. Lohr,et al.  Quantitative assessment of motor abnormalities in untreated patients with major depressive disorder. , 2013, Journal of affective disorders.

[8]  Robert D. Latzman,et al.  The Factor Structure and Age-Related Factorial Invariance of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) , 2010, Assessment.

[9]  T. Carew,et al.  Impairment in category fluency in ischemic vascular dementia. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[10]  Paul E. Croarkin,et al.  Psychomotor retardation in depression: Biological underpinnings, measurement, and treatment , 2011, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry.

[11]  G. Alexopoulos Pharmacotherapy for late-life depression. , 2011, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[12]  Anand R. Kumar,et al.  Preliminary analysis of age of illness onset effects on symptom profiles in major depressive disorder , 2013, International journal of geriatric psychiatry.

[13]  E. Kaplan,et al.  Clock Drawing Errors in Dementia: Neuropsychological and Neuroanatomical Considerations , 2004, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[14]  W. Hulstijn,et al.  Differential patterns of psychomotor functioning in unmedicated melancholic and nonmelancholic depressed patients. , 2004, Journal of psychiatric research.

[15]  K. Lange,et al.  The impact of tricyclic antidepressants and selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors on handwriting movements of patients with depression , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[16]  M. Hamilton A RATING SCALE FOR DEPRESSION , 1960, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[17]  M. Delazer,et al.  Clock Drawing, Clock Reading, Clock Setting, and Judgment of Clock Faces in Elderly People with Dementia and Depression , 2004, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[18]  D. Libon,et al.  Further analyses of clock drawings among demented and nondemented older subjects. , 1996, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[19]  B. Pollock,et al.  The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. , 2004, Archives of general psychiatry.

[20]  Charles J. Golden,et al.  Stroop Color and Word Test , 1978 .

[21]  P. Rabins,et al.  Depression without sadness: alternative presentations of depression in late life. , 1999, American family physician.

[22]  P R Houck,et al.  Decreased working memory and processing speed mediate cognitive impairment in geriatric depression , 2000, Psychological Medicine.

[23]  L. Radloff The CES-D Scale , 1977 .

[24]  W Hulstijn,et al.  Retardation in depression: assessment by means of simple motor tasks. , 1999, Journal of affective disorders.

[25]  D. Brody,et al.  Depression in the United States household population, 2005-2006. , 2008, NCHS data brief.

[26]  G. Alexopoulos,et al.  'Vascular depression' hypothesis. , 1997, Archives of general psychiatry.

[27]  M. Thase,et al.  Cognitive disturbance in outpatient depressed younger adults: evidence of modest impairment , 2001, Biological Psychiatry.

[28]  R. Martínez-Arias,et al.  [Cognitive performance of right-handed and left-handed persons on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III)]. , 2006, Revista de neurologia.

[29]  B. Christensen,et al.  Measuring premorbid IQ in traumatic brain injury: An examination of the validity of the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) , 2008, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[30]  S. Eickhoff,et al.  Internally vs. externally triggered movements in patients with major depression , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  H. Sackeim,et al.  Psychomotor symptoms of depression. , 1997, The American journal of psychiatry.

[32]  D. Kupfer,et al.  How common is resistance to treatment in recurrent, nonpsychotic geriatric depression? , 1998, The American journal of psychiatry.

[33]  Psychomotor retardation in elderly depressed patients. , 2004, Journal of affective disorders.

[34]  Linda Teri,et al.  6/Use of the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale with Older Adults , 1986 .

[35]  S. Zehetmayer,et al.  The Influence of Depression on Processing Speed and Executive Function in Nondemented Subjects Aged 75 , 2011, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[36]  R. de Raedt,et al.  Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis. , 2011, Clinical psychology review.

[37]  W. Hulstijn,et al.  No psychomotor slowing in fine motor tasks in dysthymia. , 2004, Journal of affective disorders.

[38]  D. Royall,et al.  CLOX: an executive clock drawing task , 1998, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[39]  D. Libon,et al.  Clock drawing as an assessment tool for dementia. , 1993, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[40]  J. Kelsoe,et al.  A quantitative neuromotor predictor of antidepressant non-response in patients with major depression. , 2003, Journal of affective disorders.

[41]  K. Oedegaard,et al.  Impairment across executive functions in recurrent major depression , 2004, Nordic journal of psychiatry.

[42]  M. Petrides,et al.  Differential activation of the human orbital, mid-ventrolateral, and mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the processing of visual stimuli , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.