The scaling of information to action in visually guided braking.

Braking to avoid a collision can be controlled by keeping the deceleration required to stop (i.e., ideal deceleration) in the "safe" region below maximum deceleration, but maximum deceleration is not optically specified and can vary as conditions change. When brake strength was manipulated between participants using a simulated braking task, the ratio of ideal to maximum deceleration at brake onset was invariant across groups, suggesting that calibration involves scaling information about ideal deceleration in intrinsic units of maximum deceleration. Evidence of rapid recalibration was found when brake strength was manipulated within participants, and the presence of external forces that affect brake dynamics resulted in biases in performance. Discussion focuses on the role of calibration, internal models, and affordance perception in visually guided action.

[1]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  The Role of Effort in Perceiving Distance , 2003, Psychological science.

[2]  M K Kaiser,et al.  How baseball outfielders determine where to run to catch fly balls. , 1995, Science.

[3]  W H Warren,et al.  Visual control of braking: a test of the tau hypothesis. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  J. Wann,et al.  Steering with or without the flow: is the retrieval of heading necessary? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  M. Turvey Affordances and Prospective Control: An Outline of the Ontology , 1992 .

[6]  J P Wann,et al.  Time-to-contact judgment in the locomotion of adults and preschool children. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual Guidance of Intercepting a Moving Target on Foot , 2004, Perception.

[8]  David N. Lee,et al.  VISUAL CONTROL OF VELOCITY OF APPROACH BY PIGEONS WHEN LANDING , 1993 .

[9]  D. N. Lee,et al.  Aerial docking by hummingbirds , 1991, Naturwissenschaften.

[10]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[11]  Seville Chapman Catching a Baseball , 1968 .

[12]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Calibration, information, and control strategies for braking to avoid a collision. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  W. Warren,et al.  Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  John P. Wann,et al.  Why you should look where you are going , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  H. Sedgwick The visible horizon: A potential source of visual information for the perception of size and distance. , 1973 .

[16]  J. Loomis,et al.  Visually Controlled Locomotion: Its Dependence on Optic Flow, Three-Dimensional Space Perception, and Cognition , 1998 .

[17]  C. Michaels,et al.  To Cross or Not to Cross: The Effect of Locomotion on Street-Crossing Behavior , 1996 .

[18]  J. Flach,et al.  Collisions: Getting Them Under Control , 2004 .

[19]  L. S. Mark,et al.  Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  M. Lenoir,et al.  Intercepting Moving Objects During Self-Motion. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[21]  W. Warren Action modes and laws of control for the visual guidance of action , 1988 .

[22]  B G Bardy,et al.  Visual control of braking in goal-directed action and sport. , 1997, Journal of sports sciences.

[23]  David N. Lee,et al.  A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.

[24]  G J Andersen,et al.  Speed, size, and edge-rate information for the detection of collision events. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  George J. Andersen,et al.  Chapter 6 Optical information for collision detection during deceleration , 2004 .

[26]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  Shedding some light on catching in the dark: perceptual mechanisms for catching fly balls. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  W H Warren,et al.  Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  Rich Gossweiler,et al.  Perceiving geographical slant , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[29]  M K Kaiser,et al.  Things that go bump in the light: on the optical specification of contact severity. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: perception of catchableness of fly balls. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  Gilles Montagne,et al.  Movement reversals in ball catching , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  F. C. Bakker,et al.  Catching balls: how to get the hand to the right place at the right time. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  William H. Warren,et al.  Visually Controlled Locomotion: 40 years Later , 1998 .