The Negative Consequences of Maximizing in Friendship Selection

Previous studies have shown that the maximizing orientation, reflecting a motivation to select the best option among a given set of choices, is associated with various negative psychological outcomes. In the present studies, we examined whether these relationships extend to friendship selection and how the number of options for friends moderated these effects. Across 5 studies, maximizing in selecting friends was negatively related to life satisfaction, positive affect, and self-esteem, and was positively related to negative affect and regret. In Study 1, a maximizing in selecting friends scale was created, and regret mediated the relationships between maximizing and well-being. In a naturalistic setting in Studies 2a and 2b, the tendency to maximize among those who participated in the fraternity and sorority recruitment process was negatively related to satisfaction with their selection, and positively related to regret and negative affect. In Study 3, daily levels of maximizing were negatively related to daily well-being, and these relationships were mediated by daily regret. In Study 4, we extended the findings to samples from the U.S. and Japan. When participants who tended to maximize were faced with many choices, operationalized as the daily number of friends met (Study 3) and relational mobility (Study 4), the opportunities to regret a decision increased and further diminished well-being. These findings imply that, paradoxically, attempts to maximize when selecting potential friends is detrimental to one’s well-being.

[1]  R. Larsen,et al.  The Satisfaction with Life Scale , 1985, Journal of personality assessment.

[2]  E. Diener,et al.  Re-examining the general positivity model of subjective well-being: the discrepancy between specific and global domain satisfaction. , 2001, Journal of personality.

[3]  Jason W. Osborne,et al.  Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. , 2005 .

[4]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  The Maximization Inventory , 2012, Judgment and Decision Making.

[5]  M. Yuki,et al.  Relational mobility: A socioecological approach to personal relationships. , 2012 .

[6]  Brandon M. Turner,et al.  Studies of the dimensionality, correlates, and meaning of measures of the maximizing tendency , 2011, Judgment and Decision Making.

[7]  K. Takemura,et al.  [Construction of a Japanese version of the "Regret and Maximization Scale" in decision making]. , 2008, Shinrigaku kenkyu : The Japanese journal of psychology.

[8]  Alan C. Mikkelson,et al.  Maximizing Relationship Possibilities: Relational Maximization in Romantic Relationships , 2013, The Journal of social psychology.

[9]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models , 2008, Behavior research methods.

[10]  C. B. Colby The weirdest people in the world , 1973 .

[11]  Dev K. Dalal,et al.  Understanding the Construct of Maximizing Tendency: A Theoretical and Empirical Evaluation , 2015 .

[12]  J. Nezlek Multilevel Modeling for Social and Personality Psychology , 2011 .

[13]  A. Meade,et al.  Identifying careless responses in survey data. , 2012, Psychological methods.

[14]  Engin A. Ege,et al.  Refining the measurement of maximization: Gender invariance and relation to psychological well-being , 2014 .

[15]  P. Shrout Measurement reliability and agreement in psychiatry , 1998, Statistical methods in medical research.

[16]  Dawn Iacobucci,et al.  Introduction to Mediation , 2019, Mediation Theory and Practice.

[17]  John B. Nezlek,et al.  Multilevel Random Coefficient Analyses of Event- and Interval-Contingent Data in Social and Personality Psychology Research , 2001 .

[18]  R. Curtis,et al.  Believing another likes or dislikes you: Behaviors making the beliefs come true. , 1986 .

[19]  J. Nezlek,et al.  Affect- and Self-Based Models of Relationships between Daily Events and Daily Well-Being , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[20]  J. Nezlek,et al.  Day-to-Day Relationships among Self-Concept Clarity, Self-Esteem, Daily Events, and Mood , 2001 .

[21]  M. Yuki,et al.  Relational Mobility Explains Between- and Within-Culture Differences in Self-Disclosure to Close Friends , 2010, Psychological science.

[22]  S. Armeli,et al.  Multilevel daily process designs for consulting and clinical psychology: a preface for the perplexed. , 1999, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[23]  Gergana Y. Nenkov,et al.  A short form of the Maximization Scale: Factor structure, reliability and validity studies , 2008, Judgment and Decision Making.

[24]  S. Schwartz A Theory of Cultural Values and Some Implications for Work , 1999 .

[25]  Barry Schwartz,et al.  On the meaning and measurement of maximization , 2016, Judgment and Decision Making.

[26]  M. Rosenberg Society and the adolescent self-image , 1966 .

[27]  Andrew M. Parker,et al.  Choosing to be happy? Age differences in "maximizing" decision strategies and experienced emotional well-being. , 2016, Psychology and aging.

[28]  J. Nezlek Diary methods for personality and social psychology , 2012 .

[29]  Raffaella Misuraca,et al.  The Decision Making Tendency Inventory: A New Measure to Assess Maximizing, Satisficing, and Minimizing , 2015 .

[30]  Andrew L. Geers,et al.  Choosing Across Cultures: The Effect of Choice Complexity on Treatment Outcomes , 2015 .

[31]  B. Schwartz,et al.  Does Choice Mean Freedom and Well‐Being? , 2010 .

[32]  Michael Neblo Meaning and Measurement , 2009 .

[33]  A. Hayes An Index and Test of Linear Moderated Mediation , 2015, Multivariate behavioral research.

[34]  I. Altman,et al.  Social penetration: The development of interpersonal relationships , 1973 .

[35]  B. Schwartz,et al.  The tyranny of choice: a cross-cultural investigation of maximizing-satisficing effects on well-being , 2012, Judgment and Decision Making.

[36]  Daniel J. Bauer,et al.  The disaggregation of within-person and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[37]  J. Nezlek,et al.  Distinguishing affective and non-affective reactions to daily events. , 2005, Journal of personality.

[38]  D. Mackinnon,et al.  Equivalence of the Mediation, Confounding and Suppression Effect , 2000, Prevention Science.

[39]  L. Carstensen Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity theory. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[40]  W. Fleeson Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: traits as density distribution of states. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[41]  P. Todd,et al.  Can There Ever Be Too Many Options? A Meta-Analytic Review of Choice Overload , 2010 .

[42]  H. Simon,et al.  Rational choice and the structure of the environment. , 1956, Psychological review.

[43]  Daniel J. Bauer,et al.  Conceptualizing and testing random indirect effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: new procedures and recommendations. , 2006, Psychological methods.

[44]  C. Eggleston,et al.  Are maximizers unhappier than satisficers? A comparison between Japan and the USA , 2014 .

[45]  H. Kelley,et al.  Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdependence , 1978 .

[46]  Edward F. Deroche Educating hearts and minds , 1997 .

[47]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  A general multilevel SEM framework for assessing multilevel mediation. , 2010, Psychological methods.

[48]  Francisco Álvarez,et al.  Choice Overload, Satisficing Behavior, and Price Distribution in a Time Allocation Model , 2014 .

[49]  J. Nezlek A practical guide to understanding reliability in studies of within-person variability , 2017 .

[50]  B. Schwartz,et al.  Doing Better but Feeling Worse , 2006, Psychological science.

[51]  G. Haynes,et al.  Testing the boundaries of the choice overload phenomenon: The effect of number of options and time pressure on decision difficulty and satisfaction , 2009 .

[52]  B. Schwartz,et al.  Maximizing Versus Satisficing : Happiness Is a Matter of Choice , 2002 .

[53]  Cornelia Betsch,et al.  Lieber die Taube auf dem Dach! Eine Skala zur Erfassung interindividueller Unterschiede in der Maximierungstendenz , 2006 .

[54]  The dynamics of daily events and well-being across cultures: when less is more. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[55]  P. Baltes,et al.  Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. , 1990 .

[56]  Kristopher J Preacher,et al.  Alternative Methods for Assessing Mediation in Multilevel Data: The Advantages of Multilevel SEM , 2011 .

[57]  Barry Schwartz,et al.  The Maximization Paradox : The costs of seeking alternatives , 2009 .

[58]  Richard P. Eibach,et al.  Failing to commit: Maximizers avoid commitment in a way that contributes to reduced satisfaction , 2012 .