Unilateral Right Parietal Damage Leads to Bilateral Deficit for High-Level Motion
暂无分享,去创建一个
Patrick Cavanagh | Lorella Battelli | Marie-Anne Hénaff | James Intriligator | P. Cavanagh | J. Barton | F. Michèl | J. Intriligator | L. Battelli | M. Tramo | M. Hénaff | Jason J.S. Barton | Mark J. Tramo | François Michèl
[1] M. Rizzo,et al. Motion and shape perception in cerebral akinetopsia. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[2] K. Nakayama,et al. Intact “biological motion” and “structure from motion” perception in a patient with impaired motion mechanisms: A case study , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[3] R. Rafal,et al. A systematic study of visual extinction. Between- and within-field deficits of attention in hemispatial neglect. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[4] A. Treisman,et al. Attention, Space, and Action: Studies in Cognitive Neuroscience , 2001 .
[5] P. Cavanagh,et al. Effect of surface medium on visual search for orientation and size features , 1990 .
[6] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[7] P. Cavanagh,et al. The Spatial Resolution of Visual Attention , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] A. Cowey,et al. Impairment of the perception of second order motion but not first order motion in a patient with unilateral focal brain damage , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[9] T Mergner,et al. Visual short-term memory of stimulus velocity in patients with unilateral posterior brain damage , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[10] C. Kennard,et al. Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients , 1997, Nature.
[11] Andrew T. Smith,et al. Visual detection of motion , 1994 .
[12] F. J. Friedrich,et al. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of visual attention , 1984 .
[13] S. Yantis. Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] M. Corbetta,et al. A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.
[15] A. Treisman,et al. Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST , 1997, Neuron.
[16] A. Nobre,et al. Where and When to Pay Attention: The Neural Systems for Directing Attention to Spatial Locations and to Time Intervals as Revealed by Both PET and fMRI , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] G Vallar,et al. Anatomical correlates of visual and tactile extinction in humans: a clinical CT scan study. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[18] J. Mattingley,et al. Visual extinction and prior entry: Impaired perception of temporal order with intact motion perception after unilateral parietal damage , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[19] F. J. Friedrich,et al. Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[20] O. Braddick. A short-range process in apparent motion. , 1974, Vision research.
[21] W. James,et al. The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .
[22] R Meuli,et al. Two types of auditory neglect. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[23] N. Mai,et al. Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[24] M. Wertheimer,et al. A source book of Gestalt psychology. , 1939 .
[25] J. Pokorny. Foundations of Cyclopean Perception , 1972 .
[26] G. Mather. Motion detector models: psychophysical evidence , 1994 .
[27] S Ullman,et al. Two Dimensionality of the Correspondence Process in Apparent Motion , 1978, Perception.
[28] J. Mattingley,et al. Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998, Nature.
[29] M. Corbetta,et al. Areas Involved in Encoding and Applying Directional Expectations to Moving Objects , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[30] Shimon Ullman,et al. Short- and long-range processes in structure-from-motion , 1991, Vision Research.
[31] P. Cavanagh,et al. Position-based motion perception for color and texture stimuli: effects of contrast and speed , 1999, Vision Research.
[32] Josef Ternus,et al. The problem of phenomenal identity. , 1938 .
[33] Anders M. Dale,et al. Representation of motion boundaries in retinotopic human visual cortical areas , 1997, Nature.
[34] A. Pascual-Leone,et al. Induction of visual extinction by rapid‐rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of parietal lobe , 1994, Neurology.
[35] P. Cavanagh,et al. Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness , 1996, Nature.
[36] K. Nakayama,et al. The characteristics of residual motion perception in the hemifield contralateral to lateral occipital lesions in humans. , 1993, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[37] R. Knight,et al. Cortical Networks Underlying Mechanisms of Time Perception , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[38] Rhodri Cusack,et al. Neglect Between but Not Within Auditory Objects , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[39] Curtis L. Baker,et al. Dependence on stimulus onset asynchrony in apparent motion: Evidence for two mechanisms , 1993, Vision Research.
[40] Á. Pascual-Leone,et al. Enhanced visual spatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced 'virtual lesions' of human parietal cortex , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[41] W. Newsome,et al. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[42] George Sperling,et al. Three Systems for Visual Motion Perception , 1996 .
[43] A. Cowey,et al. The selective impairment of the perception of first-order motion by unilateral cortical brain damage , 1998, Visual Neuroscience.
[44] D. Spinelli,et al. Perception of moving and stationary gratings in brain damaged patients with unilateral spatial neglect , 1992, Neuropsychologia.
[45] P. Cavanagh,et al. Attentional resolution , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[46] P. Cavanagh,et al. Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] V. Ramachandran,et al. Displacement thresholds for coherent apparent motion in random dot-patterns , 1983, Vision Research.
[48] A. Treisman,et al. The Interaction of Spatial and Object Pathways: Evidence from Balint's Syndrome , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[49] George Sperling,et al. The information available in brief visual presentations. , 1960 .
[50] G. Orban,et al. The kinetic occipital region in human visual cortex. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.
[51] P Cavanagh,et al. Attention-based motion perception. , 1992, Science.
[52] M. Corbetta,et al. A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[53] P. Cavanagh,et al. Position displacement, not velocity, is the cue to motion detection of second-order stimuli , 1998, Vision Research.
[54] A. T. Smith,et al. Detection and Discrimination of First- and Second-Order Motion in Patients with Unilateral Brain Damage , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[55] Z W Pylyshyn,et al. Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.
[56] J. Duncan,et al. Systematic analysis of deficits in visual attention. , 1999 .
[57] P. Cavanagh,et al. Motion: the long and short of it. , 1989, Spatial vision.
[58] S. Anstis. The perception of apparent movement. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[59] Alan Cowey,et al. Temporal aspects of visual search studied by transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[60] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Limits of attentive tracking reveal temporal properties of attention , 2000, Vision Research.